Thesis Nurs. 1995 C776e c. 2 Cooper. Diann C. Education's effect on anxiety in outpatient 1995. Education’s Effects Education’s Effects on Anxiety in Outpatient Cardiac Catheterization Patients by Diann C. Cooper, RN, BSN Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Master of Science in Nursing Degree Running head: EDUCATION’S EFFECTS ON ANXIETY Approved by: Chairpers^h, j^n, Thesi; Thesis Committee Edinboro University of Pennsylvania Committee Member Committee Member w 1 /Date z 1^ ms Date /?, /W? Date Education’s Effects ii Abstract Cardiac catheterizations are known to be anxiety provoking for patients. Originally an inpatient procedure, many are now outpatient procedures. The purpose of this study was to determine the effect that education, provided prior to the cardiac catheterization, had on pre-cardiac catheterization anxiety in outpatients. This correlational study used a convenience sampling method. Fourteen males between the ages of 30 and 65 were given the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory prior to receiving, and at the conclusion of, their pre-cardiac catheterization education. The results were analyzed using the Student’s t-Test. The State Anxiety scores decreased from pretest to post test. The Trait Anxiety scores increased from pretest to post test. These results were not statistically significant. Only the decrease in State Anxiety scores was supported by the literature, indicating education had an effect on anxiety. This study should be repeated with a larger sample size and with women. Education’s Effects iii Acknowledgements This study was completed through the patience, understanding and support of many people. I am truly indebted to each of them. Thank you, Brad, for the encouragement, support, reviewing , baby sitting and so much more. I love you more than words can say. Thanks, Dr. Keller, Ellen, and Dr. Beckman, for agreeing to be on my committee and for the numerous reviews, suggestions and so on that are involved with that commitment. Thanks, Deb, for reading and commenting on this study, and to Mom and Dad, just for being there. Thanks, LaMar, for explaining statistics easily and helping out with the data analysis for this study. Thanks, Jean, for your thoughtful suggestions and encouragement during this project. Thanks, also to Mary Beth, Al and all the others associated with Education and Human Resources, both past and present, for your ideas and support. Thanks to many, many friends who gave up parties, music, and vacation time to let me work on this study. And thanks for enforcing much needed breaks later on! Education’s Effects iv Table of Contents ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS iii TABLE OF CONTENTS iv LIST OF TABLES vi CHAPTER I. n. in. INTRODUCTION 1 Background of the problem 1 Purpose of the study 2 Statement of the problem 2 Assumptions 2 Definitions 3 Limitations 4 REVIEW OF LITERATURE 5 Anxiety 6 Education and Anxiety 7 Conceptual Framework 18 METHODOLOGY 20 Education’s Effects v IV. V. Sample and Setting 20 Instrumentation 21 Collection of Data 26 Analysis of Data 28 PRESENTATION AND ANALYSIS OF DATA 29 Presentation of Data 29 Analysis of Data 30 CONCLUSION 36 Conclusions 36 Recommendations 39 APPENDICES 41 Appendix A - Letter requesting research tool 41 Appendix B - Research Tool 42 Appendix C - Consent Form for Physicians 44 Appendix D - Participant Consent Form 45 Appendix E - Patient Education Booklet 46 REFERENCES 47 Education’s Effects vi List of Tables TABLE 1 - Number of Valid Observations 39 TABLE 2 - Paired Samples Student’s t-Test—State Anxiety 40 TABLE 3 - Paired Samples Student’s t-Test—Trait Anxiety 41 Education’s Effects 1 Chapter I Introduction Background of the Problem Hospitalization can be a stressful experience. As Swindale (1989) says "If unrecognized, prolonged anxiety creates stress which may subsequently harm the patient and delay recovery" (p. 899). Helping patients cope with stressful events is recognized as one of the nurse’s most vital and special responsibilities (WilsonBarnett, 1980 in Swindale, 1989). Nursing interventions that provide education and information to hospitalized patients can decrease patient anxiety. Cardiac catheterizations can be seen as stressful procedures. Though cardiac catheterizations originally were performed in the hospital, many are now being performed as an outpatient procedure. As the number of these out patient cardiac catheterization programs continues to increase, it is important for the nurse to make certain that the patients choosing this alternative receive the same quality care as inpatients would. By providing education about their procedure to patients, the nurses may decrease the patient’s anxiety and prepare them for Education’s Effects 2 discharge. The lack of this pre-procedural information may result in increased anxiety, making it more difficult for the patient to participate in his or her care and possibly resulting in post procedure complications. Purpose of the Study The purpose of this study is to determine the effect that education, provided prior to the cardiac catheterization, has on pre-cardiac catheterization anxiety. Statement of the Problem There is a positive correlation between patient anxiety levels in outpatient cardiac catheterization patients before they receive education and their anxiety levels after the education has been provided. Assumptions 1. Hospitalization is anxiety provoking in patients. Education’s Effects 3 2. Invasive and non-invasive diagnostic procedures cause anxiety. 3. Pre-procedure education decreases anxiety in inpatient cardiac catheterization patients and should decrease anxiety in outpatients as well. 4. Cardiac catheterizations will produce elevations in anxiety. 5. Participants can read and speak English. Definitions 1. Education—information about the cardiac catheterization that is given to patients about to undergo this procedure. This includes information about the procedure (procedural), information about what the patient may feel during the procedure (sensory), and information about ways to relax (coping). 2. Anxiety—a feeling of apprehension, worry, uneasiness, ordread, especially of the future. 3. Outpatient- a person who is scheduled to have a procedure done in the hospital but is not admitted to the hospital. 4. Cardiac catheterization-an invasive procedure that involves the passage of a tiny plastic tube into the heart through a blood vessel. Samples of blood are withdrawn for testing; blood pressure and cardiac output are measured. Used in Education’s Effects 4 diagnosis of heart disease and anomalies. (Taber’s Cyclopedic Medical Dictionary, 1981, p. C-19). Limitations 1. This study was limited to a sample size of thirty. 2. This study was limited to males, between the ages of 30 and 65. 3. The study was limited to a 550 bed non-profit hospital in Northwestern Pennsylvania, and to the Northeastern United States. 4. Participants can read and speak English. Education’s Effects 5 Chapter n Review of the Literature Current literature states that hospitalization is generally considered to be a stressful experience for individuals. Miller (1981) reviewed the Life Events Scales and described the Hospital Stress Rating Scale. This scale, developed by Volicer and Bohannon (1973, 1975), allows patients to place stressful life events in order. The rank of the events provides the mechanism by which the psychosocial stress experienced by patients can be measured (Miller, 1981, p. 318). Though Miller recommended further research using the scale, it is one example of hospitalization being considered stressful. Mishel (1984) investigated the relationship between perceived uncertainty and the perception of hospital events as stressful. He found that there was a strong relationship between uncertainty and stress due to vagueness, lack of clarity, and lack of information about events occurring to the patient while in the hospital. Mishel recommended that the "patients age, the recency of prior hospitalization, and seriousness of illness by disease label be evaluated upon admission. Uncertainty can be assessed during the first few days of hospital stay and the reassessed" later (p. 170-71). Education’s Effects 6 Mishel suggests nurses should provide information to the patient to reduce stress. Aguilera and Messick (1974) also saw physical illness as a stressful event which can precipitate a crisis. Anxiety is one of the first reactions to a stressful event. This is supported by Aguilera and Messick in an example of a physical illness: "Heart Disease is closely associated with death and, as a result, the patient usually first reacts with fear and anxiety." (p. 86). Anxiety When under stress, the body reacts with the ’fight or flight’ response. In brief, increased amounts of epinephrine are released from the adrenal glands leading to increases in heart rate, respiration, blood pressure, and glucose production. Blood is shunted away from less vital areas (peripheral areas) to vital areas, such as the brain. The skin becomes cool, clammy, the mouth becomes dry, the pupils dilate, and thought processes are enhanced. The individual also feels anxious. The body continues to respond in this manner until the source of stress is removed or until it can respond no more. According to Taber’s Cyclopedic Medical Dictionary (1981), anxiety is "a feeling of apprehension, worry, uneasiness, or dread, especially of the future" Education’s Effects 7 (p. A-103). It can also be described as a response to a perceived threat to the self (Roy, 1984, p. 355). In the context of the hospital environment, that threat can take the form of major or minor surgery, fear of procedures, fear of pain, fear of the unknown, and so on. Swindale (1989) quotes Wilson-Barnett (1981) as defming anxiety as "fear of the unknown, as disproportionate to the threat involved, related to the future" (p. 899). Since a variety of events occur at a hospital or during a hospital stay, there is a potential for the individual to be continually anxious. This constant state of anxiety means the body is ’geared up’ at all times. This can be detrimental to the body, and could lead to longer lengths of stay, postoperative complications, and other problems. The nurse today must be alert for the signs of anxiety in the patient and work to decrease the amount of anxiety the patient feels. Education and Anxiety Current literature supports the idea that education can have a positive effect on anxiety. Anderson (1987) studied 60 male coronary artery bypass graft patients to determine what type of preparation is most effective for patients Education’s Effects 8 undergoing cardiac surgery, by what mechanisms do preparations reduce psychological distress, and whether psychological factors influence the incidence of acute postoperative hypertension. He found that the group receiving information and the group receiving information plus coping mechanisms both had lower levels of anxiety postoperatively. These groups also recovered faster than the control group and had lower incidences of acute postoperative hypertension. Oberle, Wry, Paul, and Grace (1990) studied the relationships between anxiety, environment and postoperative pain and found that there was a strong relationship between postoperative anxiety and pain (high anxiety associated with increased pain). Bowman (1992) studied the relationship between anxiety and the development of postoperative delirium. She found that the unplanned and unexpected surgeries were associated with higher anxiety and higher postoperative delirium rates, especially if the surgery was orthopedic in nature. Cochran (1984) conducted a review of the literature with regards to psychological preparation of patients for surgical procedures. In her introduction she indicates that hospitalization provokes more anxiety for the surgical patient than for the medical patient. Her research indicates that patients who are given emotional support and information about the procedure are more cooperative and have smoother courses of recovery. Education’s Effects 9 Preoperative teaching has been evaluated at different times. Cuppies (1991) studied the effects of providing preoperative education up to 14 days before admission to the hospital and compared this group with one that received education after their admission to the hospital. She looked at preoperative knowledge of coronary artery bypass graft surgery, postoperative anxiety, postoperative mood state and physiologic recovery. She found that the experimental group, who received preoperative education prior to admission, did report less postoperative anxiety, though the difference was not statistically significant. Also, the experimental group had more positive mood states after surgery and more favorable physiologic recovery states than the control groups. Felton, Huss, Payne, and Srsic (1976) randomly assigned patients to three groups which received preoperative education. The frequency of postoperative complications, ventilatory function, anxiety level, and patients’ perceptions of psychological well being were assessed after the surgery. The experimental group, which received education about equipment used, various sites used for surgery and so on, had significantly lower levels of anxiety and higher scores on three measures of psychological well being than the other two groups. All three groups were the same with regard to the number of postoperative complications and measures of vital capacity. Brown (1990) measured anxiety pre and post Education’s Effects 10 operatively in patients undergoing renal surgery and noted that the anxiety experienced by the patients was related to the need for general anesthetic preoperatively, and pain postoperatively. She recommends providing more preoperative information to decrease the anxiety patients experience. Hathaway (1986) conducted a meta-analysis on 68 studies which examined the effect of preoperative instruction on postoperative outcomes. The results suggest that nurses need to assess the patients’ level of anxiety more closely and adapt their preoperative teaching accordingly. She found that procedural content should be provided when the patient has a lower level of fear/anxiety, and psychological content should be provided when patients have higher levels of fear/anxiety. Hathaway also analyzed the way in which the teaching was organized (structured and unstructured, individual and group), and found that individual instruction that included aspects of both structured and unstructured content was more favorable. Thus, not only procedural content but also psychological content should be included. Hathaway concludes that 67% of the patients receiving preoperative instruction have more favorable outcomes, and these outcomes are 20% better than those not receiving preoperative instruction. Education decreases anxiety as demonstrated by the following journal articles. Sheridan, Humfleet, Phair, and Lyons (1990) demonstrated that AIDS Education’s Effects 11 education decreases anxieties of community leaders. Sigsbee and Geden (1990) demonstrated that family members of patients who had coronary artery disease and were hospitalized who learned CPR had decreases in their anxiety levels after completing the CPR classes. Sirles, Brown, Hilyer (1991) found that depression, anxiety, and perceptions of pain were decreased after patients with back injuries attended a Back School. Lockard (1989) found that nursing students who participated in a death education instructional unit had reduced death anxiety as compared to those nursing students who did not participate in the unit. This result was maintained up to one year post study, as well. Hill and Balk (1987) found that stress was reduced in families of the chronically mentally ill by attending supportive education classes. Duryee (1992) conducted a literature review dealing with inpatient education after a myocardial infarction and found that, despite the anxiety associated with an MI, patients can learn new information on risk factors, symptoms, home activity, and medications. Several studies demonstrated no reduction in anxiety in the participants. For example, Zeidner, Klingman, and Papko (1988) studied the effect of a health education program on students’ test coping skills and found that, though the program increased the teachers awareness of test anxiety, the students selfreported test anxiety did not decrease. This study was conducted in Israel. Peace Education’s Effects 12 and Vincent (1988) studied death anxiety education in Hospice caregivers and found that the level of death anxiety was no different from their non-Hospice caregiver counterparts, even though the level of death education for the Hospice caregivers was much higher. Glass (1990) found that death education in elementary schools did not affect the childrens’ death anxiety levels after the education or two months later. Glass suggested that discussions of losses might have interfered with the results, as death and dying education were not the only topics covered. This study also may not be generalized due to the fact that the study group was made up of children. Besides preoperative education and educational classes, there are several other forms of education which have been shown to exert an effect on anxiety. Cassileth, Heiberger, March, and Sutton-Smith (1982) studied the effect of an audiovisual cancer program on patients and families. They found that such a program can increase the patients’ understanding of their disease, decrease anxiety and improve communication between patients, families, and physicians. Gagliano (1988) conducted a literature review of the efficacy of video in patient education and found that video programs increase short term knowledge and instruct as well as or better than other methods. When they are applied to "well- defined, self-limited, stressful situations, video modeling decreases anxiety, pain Education’s Effects 13 and sympathetic arousal while increasing knowledge, cooperation, and overall coping ability." (p. 790). Stone, Wolraich, and Hillerbrand (1988) evaluated a video training program for conveying distressful information. This information related to Public Law 94-142, which deals with the education of all handicapped children. Participants were asked to answer questions regarding self-efficacy, anxiety and feelings toward working with parents of handicapped persons. They were shown the videotape(s), depending upon which group the participants were in. The researchers found that the videos decreased anxiety related to communicating distressful information to parents of handicapped persons. Robertson, Gatchel, and Fowler (1991) used a videotape to prepare patients for emergency oral surgery. Their groups were divided by sex into a treatment and control groups, for a total of four groups. The results showed that men who watched the treatment film had decreased anxiety (as measured by heart rate) than men who watched a placebo film. The opposite effect was noted in the women; the results showed decreased anxiety from the placebo film, not the treatment film. A study of 69 subjects by Friedman, Badere, and Fitzpatrick (1992) showed that those participants who watched television prior to their surgery had a significantly lower level of anxiety than those who did not. No particular program was to be watched. Education’s Effects 14 Hagopian (1991) assessed the effects of a radiation newsletter on patients. This newsletter was used to convey information about radiation therapy to patients receiving this type of therapy. She found that the newsletter increased the patients knowledge and self-care behaviors. Unfortunately, she did not assess for its effect on anxiety, she only recommended that this be addressed in future research. Hartfield, Cason, and Cason (1982) conducted a quasi-experimental study to examine the effects of sensory preparation (as opposed to procedural information) for a threatening event (barium enema) on patient expectations and emotional distress. They found that patients receiving sensory preparation for the barium enema reported significantly less anxiety and expectations more congruent with the actual experience than did those who received the procedural information alone. In an earlier study, Hartfield and Cason (1981) looked at the effect of information on emotional responses during a barium enema and found that the participants who received sensation information reported less anxiety than those who received no information or procedural information. Sime, and Libera (1985) also evaluated sensation information with regards to dental surgery. Subjects were placed in one of four groups; sensation, sensation and self-instruction, selfinstruction, and control. An audiotape provided all information. The results of Education’s Effects 15 this study showed that patients with high anxiety reported less tension and distress during surgery after receiving sensation information, less tension after receiving self-instruction information, and increased use of positive self-statements with a combination of information. Low anxiety patients did not benefit from treatment. In another study, Fullhart (1992) compared information given before sigmoidoscopy and anxiety and found that there was no difference in anxiety levels of patients before and after being given procedure information when compared with anxiety of those who received both procedure and sensation information. These results may be related to the size of the sample, according to the author. Toth (1980) examined the effect of structured preparation for transfer on patient anxiety. She compared structured with unstructured information that was given to stable myocardial infarction patients. Though the subjects who received teaching did not report feeling less anxious, the measurements of systolic blood pressure and heart rate on the day of transfer and at the time of transfer were significantly lower for the structured group. Anderson and Masur (1989) looked at psychological preparation of patients undergoing cardiac catheterization. Subjects were assigned to one of five groups: sensory-procedural information, modeling, cognitive-behavioral coping skills, Education’s Effects 16 modeling plus coping skills, or control. The results showed that modeling and modeling plus coping skills were the most effective preparatory strategies. Subjects in these groups reported less subjective anxiety, and greater perceived coping skills. The information group did not differ from the control group in these areas. Peterson (1991) studied cardiac catheterization patients as well. She looked at anxiety before a catheterization. Subjects were placed in one of three groups: educational intervention, social intervention, and control. Sensory and procedural information was provided to the education group. The social group spent the time "chatting" with the researcher. Results of this study showed no difference between the educational and social groups. The author suggests the patients’ anxiety level may be lessened by having a support person present and she advocates further research in this area. A variety of other articles exist that discuss outpatient procedures, education, and anxiety. Allen, Knight, Falk, and Strang (1992) studied the effectiveness of preoperative teaching for cataract patients. They found that a home based education program was as effective as teaching done in the hospital in preparing patients for cataract surgery. Though not statistically significant, there was also a decrease in preoperative anxiety after the teaching program was completed. Hill, Baker, Warner, and Taub (1988) studied the use of a videotape Education’s Effects 17 in teaching the pre-cardiac catheterization patient. Two groups were used, one to view the videotape and the other received the information via an interview. The results showed that both groups had lower anxiety scores following instruction and the scores did not differ between groups. The authors suggest using both the video and the interview together to prepare patients for cardiac catheterization. Warner, Peebles, Miller, Reed, Rodriquez, and Martin-Lewis (1992) studied the effectiveness in teaching a relaxation technique to inpatients undergoing elective cardiac catheterization. This study found that anxiety scores decreased for both the experimental and the control groups after the cardiac catheterization, with the decrease statistically significant only for the experimental group. The authors believe this to be due to the relaxation technique. Finesilver (1978) found that patients who received preparatory sensory information before their first cardiac catheterization were significantly less distressed during the procedure than patients who received the usual care. Verderber, Shively, and Fitzsimmons (1992) noted in their study that modeling preparation was more effective than an information intervention for cardiac catheterization patients. Kendall, Williams, et al (1979) studied cognitive-behavioral and patient education interventions in cardiac catheterization patients. The patient education and the cognitive-behavioral groups had significantly lower state anxiety scores than the Education’s Effects 18 control groups. These two groups had received formal training in relaxation or procedural information related to cardiac catheterizations. Conceptual Framework In dealing with an individual with the potential for a crisis, Aguilera and Messick (1974) advocate assessing the individual for his perception of the event, his coping mechanisms, and the available situational support to determine if the person is in crisis (p. 55-65). Roy (1984) believes in assessing the individual in all four modes (physiological, self concept, role function, and interdependence) to determine which is most disrupted by anxiety. Roy advocates a three-step process of goals and interventions that will help promote adaptive behavior. The first goal is for the individual to realize that he or she is anxious. The second goal is for the individual to identify the source of his or her anxiety. The third goal is for the individual to be able to cope with his or her anxiety (p. 365-66). These two theories compliment each other in that nurses need to make sure that the individual has a realistic perception of the event causing the anxiety, and that the individual is able to cope with the anxiety. Besides using open ended questions and providing an environment of trust, the goals of both theories can Education’s Effects 19 be reached by providing the individual with the necessary information about the situation and about alternate coping mechanisms. Kendall, Williams, et al, (1979) stated in their study of education interventions in cardiac catheterization patients that "invasive medical procedures can be viewed as crises because there is often inordinant stress and acute elevations of anxiety. Supplying the patient in crisis with effective methods to deal with stress should also improve the patient’s behavior and adjustment during the medical procedures themselves." (p. 49). Education’s Effects 20 Chapter ni Methodology This correlational study was done to determine what effect education has on the anxiety levels of outpatient cardiac catheterization patients. The purpose of this study was to determine the effect that education, provided prior to the cardiac catheterization, had on pre-cardiac catheterization anxiety. Sample and Setting The sample for this study was a convenience sample consisting of 16 male outpatients who underwent their first cardiac catheterization. The setting was the Pre-Admission Teaching and Testing area. This area was open from 7:00 am until 9:00 pm Monday through Friday. Appointments were made by the Primary Physician’s office staff for each patient that is to have a procedure done at this hospital. These patients come to the Pre-Admission area on the day of thenappointment to have all pre-operative tests completed. Pre-operative teaching takes place at this time as well. For the out patient cardiac catheterization Education’s Effects 21 patients, this teaching includes information about the One Day Stay unit, as well as the procedure. The One Day Stay unit of this medium sized not for profit hospital located in the Northeastern United States has 48 beds and operates five days a week, Monday through Friday, from 5:00 am to 9:00 pm. Patients were admitted the morning of their procedure and discharged the same day, several hours after the procedure was completed. The Cardiac Catheterization Laboratory was located in the same hospital, one floor below the One Day Stay unit. Instrumentation The instrument used for this study was the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) Form Y by Spielberger and Gorsuch (1983) (Appendix B). The first form was developed in 1964, when the authors set out to create "a single set of items that could be administered with different instructions to provide objective measures of state and trait anxiety.” (Spielberger, 1983, p. 12) The questions on both the state and trait anxiety forms were obtained from an item pool. Each of these items was related to other measures of anxiety, most commonly trait anxiety. In order to measure state anxiety, the items in the item pool were Education’s Effects 22 rewritten by the authors. The result was Form A. While studying the validity of the form, the authors discovered that some items measured both state and trait anxiety without distinguishing between them, some items were not clearly understood, and some items could not be used when the instructions were altered. In the next format, Form X, the authors chose items with the "best psychometric properties" (Spielberger, 1983, p. 12). Standardization and validity testing involved approximately 6800 people, including high school and college students, neuropsychiatric and medical and surgical patients, and prison inmates. The STAI is a 40 item self evaluation questionnaire made up of two twenty item sub scales, the State Anxiety and Trait Anxiety scales. Spielberger (1983) defines them in the following manner: "The Trait Anxiety scale refers to the differences between people in the tendency to perceive stressful situations as dangerous or threatening and to respond to such situations with increases in the intensity of their State Anxiety reactions." (p. 1) The items in each scale assess anxiety as being either present or absent within the individual. In Form X, the State Anxiety scale had equal numbers of the anxiety present and absent items. The Trait Anxiety scale did not (thirteen anxiety present and 7 anxiety absent items). In 1980, Spielberger administered Form X with twelve potential replacement items to high school students. The results led to the replacement of Education’s Effects 23 six items in each scale with items of "equal or better psychometric properties and content that was more consistent with our concepts of state and trait anxiety" (Spielberger, 1983, p. 13). This became Form Y, which is the present form. Reliability and internal consistency tests for Form Y are based on two groups of high school students. The results as reported by Spielberger (1983) are as follows: stability, as measured by test-retest coefficients, is relatively high for the STAI T-Anxiety scale and low for the S-Anxiety scale, as would be expected for a measure assessing changes in anxiety resulting from situational stress. The internal consistency for both the S-Anxiety and T-Anxiety scales are quite high as measured by alpha coefficients and item-remainder correlations. The internal consistency for Form Y is slightly higher than for Form X, which has resulted from replacement of the items in the earlier form.” (p. 14). The overall median alpha coefficients for the two scales in Form Y in the normative samples are higher than those for Form X, as well. Validity testing of the STAI occurred on two levels. The validity of the individual items was determined by administering the test to undergraduate Education’s Effects 24 college students in four settings: regular classroom, after relaxation training, following a difficult IQ exam, and after viewing a stressful movie. The item­ remainder correlation coefficients for each of the State-Anxiety (S-Anxiety) items were higher for the more stressful conditions than the relaxed conditions. As they relate to the Trait-Anxiety (T-Anxiety) items, the correlations for the SAnxiety items were slightly higher. Concurrent, convergent, divergent, and construct validity research was reported in six areas: contrasted groups, correlations between S-Anxiety and T- Anxiety scales, correlation of the T-Anxiety with other measures of trait anxiety, correlation of the STAI scales with other common measures of personality and adjustment, correlations of the STAI scales with measures of aptitude and achievement, and investigations of the effects of different amounts and types of stress on the S-Anxiety scores. For the contrasted groups, the higher T-Anxiety scores in neuropsychiatric patients proves it can discriminate between normals (working adults and students) and psychiatric patients for whom anxiety is a major symptom. T-Anxiety scores were lower in a character disorder group for whom absence of anxiety is an important symptom, as compared with the norms, and S-Anxiety scores were higher in military recruits who recently began stressful training programs versus scores of college and high school students who were Education’s Effects 25 tested under non-stressfiil conditions. These results support the construct validity for the STAI. Correlations between the S-Anxiety and T-Anxiety scales show the results to be higher under conditions that pose a threat to self esteem or personal adequacy, and are lower in situations involving personal danger. Changes in SAnxiety because of physical danger appear unrelated to the level of T-Anxiety. The T-Anxiety scale correlates highly with the IPAT and the Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale, which demonstrates evidence of concurrent validity. The STAl was correlated with the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory, the Cornell Medical Index, and the United States Army Beta intelligence test and indicate comparable correlations between the MMPI and STAl, and high correlations for both the T-Anxiety and S-Anxiety scales and the Cornell Medical Index. Several other tests were also compared with the STAI and demonstrate positive correlations. There was no relationship between the STAI and the Beta test, which means that STAI is not related to measures of intelligence or scholastic aptitude. There is no relationship between the STAI and academic aptitude and achievement, as Spielberger demonstrated using scores from Florida’s Statewide Twelfth Grade Placement test and College Entrance Examination scores. Spielberger conducted several studies to look at the effect of stress on the SAnxiety scale. He noted S-Anxiety scores were higher in exam conditions than Education’s Effects 26 for normal conditions, regardless of the participants sex. These results were repeated in subsequent studies, further supporting the construct validity of the STAI. Numerous studies have used the STAI in research or in studies that further support its reliability and validity. These were most recently compiled and published by Spielberger in The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: A Comprehensive Bibliography (1983). Permission to use the STAI was obtained from the author by ordering it from Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, California. The cost was $12 for the manual and test booklet, $7 for the scoring key and $16 for the booklets needed for the data collection. (Appendices A and B) Collection of the Data The participants were approached by the investigator at the time of admission and asked to participate in the study. AU potential participants had cardiologists who are members of the same practice. The primary cardiologist in the practice was contacted and his permission to approach patients in the group’s practice with regards to taking part in this study was obtained (Appendix Education’s Effects 27 C). The study was explained to them as being a part of a graduate student’s degree requirements. Each participant was assured of the confidentiality of their results, and informed that they would receive pre-procedure education whether they participated in the study or not. If they agreed to participate, a consent form was signed. (Appendix D) After signing the consent form, the participants were given the STAI to complete. The investigator reviewed the written instructions with each participant prior to them taking the test. After the STAI was completed and returned to the instructor, the participants were given the cardiac education booklet (Appendix E) and shown the cardiac catheterization film. The booklet reviews cardiac catheterization, the tests done prior to the catheterization, significant definitions, anatomy and physiology, what happens before, during and after the test, and what will happen when they return to their room. The film, produced by the hospital, walks the patient through the cardiac catheterization, showing the patient the catheterization laboratory and another patient undergoing the procedure while the narrator describes what the patient may feel (numbness, heat, pain) and what the patient can do to cope with these sensations and feelings (deep breaths, visualization). At the completion of the video, the participants are asked to retake the STAI, which was then collected by the investigator. Education’s Effects 28 Analysis of Data Each STAI item has a weighted score of one to four. For ten S-Anxiety and eleven T-Anxiety items, a rating of four indicates a high level of anxiety (anxiety-present). A high rating of four for the remaining ten S-Anxiety and nine T-Anxiety items, indicates the presence of low anxiety (anxiety-absent). This occurs because the weights for the anxiety absent items are based on a reversed Likert scale (response #1=4 points). The anxiety-absent items for the S Anxiety scale are numbers 1, 2, 5, 8, 10, 11, 15, 16, 19, 20. The anxiety-absent items for the T-Anxiety scale are numbers 21, 23, 26, 27, 30, 33, 34, 36, 39. Scores are obtained by adding the totals for each item on the scales together. Scores can range from 20 to 80. Norms for the STAI were established by Spielberger and are listed in the test manual. A Student’s t-Test was used to analyze the pre to post test differences in the scores for both State and Trait Anxiety. Education’s Effects 29 Chapter IV Presentation of Data The purpose of this study was to determine the effect that education, provided prior to the cardiac catheterization, had on pre-cardiac catheterization anxiety. Over a seven month period of time, a convenience sample consisting of 16 white males between the ages of 30 and 65 were interviewed and agreed to participate in the study. Their average age was 52.13. Each participant lived within the tri-state area, and traveled 60 miles or less to keep their appointment in the Pre-Admission area. Of the 16, only 14 of the STAI surveys could be used. One gentleman did not complete the entire post test and the responses of a second gentleman were not used due to his learning handicap (his father completed the pretest and post test for him). Each of the answer sheets was hand scored using the STAI answer sheet. This answer sheet takes into account the weighing of each question, and the individual scores are based upon the weighing scale discussed earlier. Each person ended up with four separate scores; a pretest State Anxiety score, a pretest Trait Anxiety score, a post test State Anxiety score, and a post test Trait Anxiety score. The pre and post test State Anxiety scores Education’s Effects 30 were compared to each other, as were the pre and post test Trait Anxiety scores. Table 1 summarizes the data collected. Table 2 lists the State Anxiety statistical results from pre to post test. Table 3 lists the Trait Anxiety statistical results from pre and post test. Analysis of Data The purpose of this study was to determine the effect that education, provided prior to the cardiac catheterization, had on pre-cardiac catheterization anxiety. The study was set up as a repeated measures design. This design allows for the same group to act as its own control. The test was given to the members of the group before and after the experimental procedure. This ensures that both groups are equal. The statistical test used to analyze the data was the two-tailed Student’s t-Test. This test was designed for small sample sizes. In order to test for significance with the smaller sample size, the Student’s t-Test can be used. This test looks at the t distribution, instead of the normal curve. The t distribution looks very much like the normal curve, except that as sample sizes get smaller the curve becomes narrower and taller in the middle (leptokurtic). This results in the tails being higher, and one must go farther out Education’s Effects 31 to find t values that correspond to the 5% and 1% areas in the distribution. As the sample size gets larger the curve looks more like the normal curve. When a sample size of 30 is reached, the curves are almost identical. As the sample size gets smaller, the curve becomes more leptokurtic. This means that a larger t value is needed to reject a null hypothesis. Significance is determined by using the degrees of freedom (df) and the t value of the sample size and comparing them to the t table. The t table lists the t values which must be equaled or surpassed for the usual significance levels of .05, .01, and .001 for various sample sizes, using degrees of freedom. Several assumptions are associated with the Student’s t-Test (Bartz, 1976). They are: 1. The scores must by interval or ratio in nature; 2. The scores must be measures on random samples from the respective populations; 3. The populations from which the samples were drawn must be normally distributed; 4. The populations from which the samples were drawn must have approximately the same variability (homogeneity or variance), (p. 253) Before the t value can be looked at as significant, the above assumptions must be met. Education’s Effects 32 In Table 2 and Table 3, the statistical test results for the State and Trait Anxiety are listed. In order for the t value to be significant at the .05 level a t value of 2.160 was needed. The t value for State (S) Anxiety was .10 and for Trait (T) Anxiety -.38. This test was two-tailed so that both positive and negative t values would be calculated. The results were not significant, which supports the null hypothesis of there is no difference between pretest and post test scores for State or Trait Anxiety. Education’s Effects 33 Table 1 Number of Valid Observations Variable Mean SD* Min Max N State Anxiety 40.07 9.22 23 54 14 Label State Anxiety- Pretest Trait Anxiety 34.71 7.80 27 53 14 Trait Anxiety- Pretest State Anxiety 39.93 8.93 23 53 14 State AnxietyPost Test Trait Anxiety 35.14 8.76 20 51 14 Trait Anxiety- Post Test *SD—Standard Deviation Education’s Effects 34 Table 2 Paired Samples Student’s t-Test-State Anxiety Variable N Mean Standard Standard Deviation Error Pretest 14 40.0714 9.219 2.464 Post Test 14 39.9286 8.931 2.387 Results Mean .1429 SD* 5.376 SE* 1.437 *SD—Standard Deviation SE-Standard Error df—Degrees of Freedom Corr .825 2 Tail t Prob Value .000 .10 df* 2 Tail Prob 13 .922 Education’s Effects 35 Table 3 Paired Samples Student’s t-Test-Trait Anxiety Variable N Mean Standard Standard Deviation Error Pretest 14 34.7143 7.800 2.085 Post Test 14 35.1429 8.761 2.341 Results Mean -.4286 SD* 4.256 SE* 1.137 *SD—Standard Deviation SE—Standard Error df—Degrees of Freedom Corr .874 2 Tail t Prob value .000 -.38 df* 2 Tail prob 13 .712 Education’s Effects 36 Chapter V Conclusion The purpose of this study was to determine the effect that education, provided prior to the cardiac catheterization, had on pre-cardiac catheterization anxiety. In response to the research question, is there a positive correlation between patient anxiety levels in outpatient cardiac catheterization patients before they receive education and their anxiety levels after the education has been provided, the following was noted. There was very little difference in the SAnxiety scores from pre to post test, though there was a slight decrease. The decrease was not enough to demonstrate significance, even with a sensitive test for small sample sizes. The decrease is supported by the literature. In the review of literature for this study, education was noted to decrease anxiety levels in a statistically significant manner (Anderson, 1987; Oberle, et.al., 1990; Bowman, 1992; Cochran, 1984; Felton, et.al. 1976; Sheridan, et.al., 1990; Sigsbee and Geden, 1990; Sides, Brown, and Hilyer, 1991; Ixickard, 1989; Hill and Balk, 1987; Duryee, 1992). However, Cuppies (1991) reported that the experimental group in her study experienced less anxiety post operatively, but that Education’s Effects 37 this decrease was not staUstic.fi, significant. Her stud, invoiced preoperadve education, offered at various times prior to the procedure, and anxiety. She surmises that the preoperative level of anxiety for CABG patients was so high that nothing would bring it down and concludes by recommending that the study be repeated. Several other studies found no reduction in anxiety after education was provided. Zeidner, Klingman, and Papko (1988) attribute the lack of significant reduction in test taking anxiety to the possibility that the students transferred some of the skills learned in the program to performance. As a result, performance improved but test taking anxiety did not decrease. Peace and Vincent (1988) suggested that the return rate of the questionnaires, sending of the questionnaires in the mail versus personal interviews and using ANA members as the source of the sample may have contributed to the lack of significance in death anxiety levels between Hospice Care Nurses (HCN) and Technical Care Nurses (TCN). They further speculated that differences in the education level of HCNs and TCNs might have had an effect on death anxiety levels and they recommend further research along this line. Glass (1990) found no change in the death anxiety scores of children after the death education classes. This he attributed to the fact that many losses were discussed in the class, not just death and dying. Also, there was the possibility that Ute following could have influenced the results: two Education’s Effects 38 weeks was not enough time for the study, that students realized they very dependent on their parents and fear the death of these people, and the students interacted with each other during the course of the school day. He also recommends further study. The T-Anxiety scores increased slightly from the pre- to the post test, although this difference was not statistically significant. The T-Anxiety score measures general anxiety. The education the participants received would not be expected to affect it. Essentially, the change indicates the participants rated themselves as more anxious in general after they saw the video. This result was not anticipated and was not supported by the literature. Several factors could play a part in these results. The sample was not a true random sample; it was a convenience sample since only those males who came through the Pre-Admission Unit who met the criteria and agreed to participate in the study were interviewed. The sample was not large enough, and results cannot be generalized to the larger population. A Type II sampling error should be considered. There was no way to guarantee that all patients scheduled to have an outpatient cardiac catheterization would or actually did come to the Pre-Admission area for their tests and teaching. This would affect the sample from which the participants were obtained. The design of the study, also, did not Education’s Effects 39 control for the Hawthorne effect. Recommendations The purpose of this study was to determine the effect that education provided prior to the cardiac catheterization, had on pre-cardiac catheterization anxiety. A positive correlation was noted between the participants’ State Anxiety test scores before they received education and their post test scores. The Trait Anxiety scores were increased from pretest to post test. Neither of these results were statistically significant. Listed below are the recommendations based upon these results. 1. This study should be repeated with a larger sample of males in the same situation. 2. All outpatient cardiac catheterization patients should be considered for the sample. 3. Random sampling technique should be used. 4. This study should be repeated with women in a similar age group who are outpatients undergoing their first cardiac catheterizations. There were some benefits to this study in that it helped to identify a Education’s Effects 40 process problem for the Pre-Admission Unit and the physician office. This problem is being addressed by the Unit Nurse Manager and several others. The study was able to show that education has an effect on anxiety, as demonstrated by the slight decrease in S-Anxiety scores. Appendix A January 25, 1994 Consulting Psychologists Press P.O. Box 10096 Palo Alto, CA 94303-0979 Dear Sir or Madam; I am a student at Edinboro University of Pennsylvania in their graduate Nursing program. My thesis, at this time, focuses on Education’s Effects on Anxiety in Outpatient Cardiac Catheterization Patients. I would like to utilize the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) as my tool. The STAI would not be altered or adapted in any way, and would be given to patients before they receive any education about the catheterization procedure and then immediately after they receive education about the procedure. Given this information, I would like to request your permission to use this tool. As directed, my Thesis committee chairperson, Dr. Mary Lou Keller, has signed this letter at the bottom. In anticipation of receiving your permission, I would like to order the following items: Cost Description Quantity Item # $12.00 Manual and Test Booklet 1 #4201 $7.00 1 Scoring key #4206 $16.00 50 Test Booklets #4217 Shipping and Handling Total (enclosed) $3.50 (10% of total) $38.50 I hope that this letter meets your requirements and that you will send the above material as soon as possible to the address listed below. If the information I have provided is not complete, please let me know so that I may send the missing information to you. My home and work phone numbers are included for your convenience. Thank you for your help. Sincerely, Diann C. Cooper, RN 6500 Avonia Road Fairview, PA 16415 phone: (H) 814-474-3188 (W) 814-877-3540 Dr. Mary Lou Keller, RN, Ph.D. Edinboro University of Pennsylvania SELF-EVALUATION QUESTIONNAIRE Developed by Charles D. Spielberger in collaboration with R. L. Gorsuch, R. Lushene, P. R. Vagg, and G. A. Jacobs STAI Form Y-l Name Age Date Sex: M S F DIRECTIONS: A number of statements which people have used to describe themselves are given below. Read each statement and then blacken in the appropriate circle to the right of the statement to indi­ cate how you feel right now, that is, at this moment. There are no right or wrong answers. Do not spend too much time on any one statement but give the answer which seems to describe your present feelings best. T___ •x' ® 1. I feel calm © © 2. I feel secure © © 3. I am tense © © © ® 4. I feel strained © © © ® 5. I feel at ease © © © ® 6. I feel upset © © ® 7. I am presently worrying over possible misfortunes © © ® © © © ® © © ® © © ® © © ® © © ® © © ® © © ® © © ® © © ® © © ® © © ® © © ® © © ® © 8. I feel satisfied 9. I feel frightened 10. I feel comfortable 11. I feel self-confident 12. I feel nervous 13. I am jittery 14. I feel indecisive 15. I am relaxed .. . 16. I feel content 17. I am worried .. 18. I feel confused . . . 19. I feel steady © ® .. - 20. I feel pleasant Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc. 3803 E. Bayshore Road • Palo Alto, CA 94303 SELF-EVALUATION QUESTIONNAIRE STAI Form Y-2 Name DIRECTIONS: A number of statements which people have used to describe themselves are given below. Read each statement and then blacken in the appropriate circle to the right of the statement to in­ dicate how you generally feel. There are no right or wrong answers. Do not spend too much time on any one statement but give the answer which seems to describe how you generally feel. Date X.-4 *p 21. I feel pleasant © ® ® 22. I feel nervous and restless © ® ® 23. I feel satisfied with myself © ® ® ® 24. I wish I could be as happy as others seem to be © ® ® ® 25. I feel like a failure ® ® ® ® 26. I feel rested © ® ® ® 27. I am “calm, cool, and collected” ® ® ® ® 28. I feel that difficulties are piling up so that I cannot overcome them © ® ® ® 29. I worry too much over something that really doesn’t matter © ® ® ® 30. I am happy © ® ® ® ® @ ® 31. I have disturbing thoughts ® 32. I lack self-confidence © ® ® ® 33. I feel secure © ® ® ® 34. I make decisions easily ® ® ® 35. I feel inadequate ® ® ® 36. I am content © ® ® ® 37. Some unimportant thought runs through my mind and bothers me © ® ® ® ® ® ® ® ® ® ® ® ® 38. I take disappointments so keenly that I can’t put them out of my mind .. . 39. I am a steady person © 40. I get in a state of tension or turmoil as I think over my recent concerns and interests . .. Copyright 1968. 1977 by Charles D. Spielberger. Reproduction of this test or any portion thereof by any process without written permission of the Publisher is prohibited. Sixteenth pnnttng. Education’s Effects 44 Appendix C Physician Approval I have been approached by Diann Cooper for permission to ask patients in my group’s practice to be included in her graduate research study. The purpose of this research investigation is to determine the effects pre-procedure education has on outpatient cardiac catheterization patient’s anxiety. The study will take place in the Pre-Admission Teaching and Testing (PATT) area. Our first time cardiac catheterization outpatients who go to this area prior to their catheterization will be asked to sign an informed consent in order to participate in this study. I give Diann Cooper, BSN, permission to ask our patients for thenconsent to participate in this study. Physician Signature Education’s Effects 45 Appendix D Informed Consent I understand that I am being asked to be a part of a study to determine the effect that education given the outpatient cardiac catheterization patients has upon their anxiety level prior to the cardiac catheterization. I understand that I have the right to refuse to be a part of this study and that if I refuse, it will not change how the nurse cares for me. I also understand that I have a right to withdraw from this study at any time. I understand that my confidentiality will be maintained and that I will be given a code number known only by the investigator. I also understand that the result will be reported as a part of a group and that I may contact the investigator, Diann Cooper, at 877-3540 if I want the results of the study. By signing my name below, I am showing that I have read and understood the above information and that I give my consent to volunteer as a research subject in this study. PATIENT SIGNATURE DATE INVESTIGATOR SIGNATURE DATE Appendix E CARDIAC CATHETERIZATION left coronary artery left main circumflex right coronary artery ' J -9 ¥ u r .octuse marginal Inside Heart s left ? / anterior descending aortic valve posterior descenoing mitral valve Outside Heart pulmonary vatve tncuspid valve CARDIAC CATHETERIZATION WHAT IS A CARDIAC CATHETERIZATION? Cardiac Catheterization is a test used to discover any abnormalities in your heart valves, chambers, major blood vessels, or fatty deposits in the coronary arteries. It is not surgery. Some people may call it different names: ■ 'Coronary Arteriogram", ■ "Coronary Angiogram", ■ "Dye study of the Heart". These all refer to the same test. WHAT IS THE PREPARATION FOR A CARDIAC CATHETERIZATION? A. Your doctor must know if you: 1. Are allergic to any medication, x-ray dye (contrast media) or food. 2. Are or may be pregnant. Cannot be flat on your back for long periods of time. 3. Have trouble urinating when you are lying down or have 4. been told you have prostate disease. Have glaucoma. 5. Have been taking aspirin, products containing aspirin or 6. blood thinners such as coumadin. Are a diabetic and if you take insulin. 7. Have or are being treated for infections in any part of your 8. body. Have had a previous cardiac catheterization. 9. Before the test, you may have: B. A chest x-ray. 1. An EKG. 2. Several routine blood tests. 3. 4. 5. 6. c. An IV started. An examination by the cardiologist (heart specialist). Hair shaved on your arm or groin. You should not have anything to eat or drink after midnight the night before your catheterization. A consent must be signed showing that you understand the procedure and give your informed consent for it to be done. Ask questions and make sure you understand the test before you sign the consent form. A Cardiac Catheterization film is available on the patient education channel. Your nurse can tell you the times it is to be shown. WHAT HAPPENS BEFORE THE TEST? You will be given medication to relax you before you go for the test. You will then be taken to the holding area of the Cardiac Catheterization Laboratory. Your chart will be reviewed and you will be asked a few questions about how you feel, allergies and past illnesses. You will then be taken to the procedure room, placed on an x-ray table and prepared for the test. Three staff members will be with you in the procedure room. One will assist the doctor, one will take care of you, answer your questions and collect data, and one will be responsible for your x-ray pictures. WHAT HAPPENS DURING THE TEST? A. Medicine will be injected to numb your skin (either at the bend in your elbow or in your groin). B. A long thin flexible tube called a catheter will be placed in a blood vessel and then guided to your heart. The catheter may be placed in either arteries ------- s or veins, depending on what information is needed. C. A special medicine called contrast dye or contrast media will be injected through the catheter into the blood vessels and heart chambers. You will feel warm all over when this is injected. The contrast medium makes it possible to see inside the vessels and heart chambers. Blockages or narrowings in the vessels can be seen. Their location is documented on movie film. The heart muscle’s ability to pump, the function of the valves between the heart chambers, and narrowings in the coronary arteries can be seen during cardiac catheterization. WHAT HAPPENS AFTER THE TEST? After the test is over, you will be taken back to the holding area A. where: Your blood pressure will be checked. 1. The catheter will be removed. 2. Pressure will be held over the puncture site until the 3. bleeding stops. A dressing will be applied to the puncture site. 4. Your pulses below the puncture site will be checked. 5. You will be given post procedure instructions. 6. B. C. An orderly will take you back to your room. You will be gone from your nursing unit for 1 1/2 to 2 hours. WE REQUEST THAT YOUR FAMILY/VISITORS REMAIN IN YOUR ROOM DURING THE PROCEDURE. This will enable us to reach them if that should become necessary. The doctor will come to your room to give you and your family the results of your test when he has had the opportunity to view the final films. WHAT HAPPENS BACK IN MY ROOM? The nurse will examine the puncture site, check your pulses and take your blood pressure. If your groin area was used, you will be asked to stay in bed for up to 6 hours, keeping your leg straight (This will prevent bleeding). A sandbag may be placed on your groin site to remind you to keep that leg straight. If your arm was used, bed rest will not be necessary, but you will need to keep your elbow straight for 6 hours (This will prevent bleeding). You can eat and drink after the test is over. Contrast dye is passed out of your body through the kidneys. Extra fluids will help your kidneys get rid of the dye. There will not be a change in the color of your urine. Please talk to your doctor or nurse if you have any questions about this test. FOR YOUR INFORMATION: A. DEFINITIONS1. VEINS- vessels that carry blood from the body back to the right side of the heart. B. 2. ARTERIES - vessels that carry blood that has been pumped out of the heart to the rest of the body. 3. CORONARY ARTERIES - vessels that carry blood to the muscle of the heart. 4. VALVES - valves are located between the heart chambers to keep the blood moving forward through the heart. 5. CONTRAST MEDIUM - a clear injectable liquid, usually called DYE, that x-rays can not penetrate (see through). 6. CORONARY BLOCKAGES - a condition which results from the buildup of fatty deposits in the walls of an artery in the heart. ANATOMY AND FUNCTION The heart is a four chambered muscular organ which 1. functions as a pump. 2. The heart muscle receives oxygen and nutrient rich blood supply from the coronary arteries. 3. The volume of blood that the heart is able to pump to the body depends upon the strength of the heart muscle’s contractions. 4. Blockages and narrowing in the coronary arteries decrease the amount of blood that the heart muscle receives. 5. Angina (heart pain) may also be felt when the heart muscle is not receiving a full blood supply. 6. The heart’s strong pumping action can be weakened when there are blockages and narrowings. A tired feeling results. 7. Weakness, fluid in the lungs and shortness of breath may result when heart valves are narrowed or leaking. 8. Cardiac catheterization gives your doctor the information he needs to diagnose and recommend treatment for your specific cardiac problems. QUESTIONS I WANT TO ASK MY CAREGIVER... PATIENT EDUCATION COMMITTEE njv/PTED/044 7/93 Education’s Effects 47 References Aguilera, D. C. & Messick, J. M. (1974). Crisis Intervention: Theory and Methodology. St. Louis: C. V. Mosby Company. Allen, M., Knight, C., Falk, C., & Strang, V. (1992). Effectiveness of a preoperative teaching program for cataract patients. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 17, 303-309. Anderson, E. A. (1987). Preoperative preparation for cardiac surgery facilitates recovery, reduces psychological distress, and reduces the incidence of acute postoperative hypertension. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 513-520. Anderson, K. O. & Masur, F. T. (1989). Psychologic preparation for cardiac catheterization. Heart & Lung, 18, 154-163. Bartz, A. E. (1976). Basic Statistical Concepts in Education and the Behavioral Sciences. Minneapolis: Burgess Publishing Company. Bowman, A. (1992). The relationship of anxiety to development of postoperative delirium. Journal of Gerontological Nursing, 18, 24-30. Brown, S. (1990). Quantitative measurement of anxiety in patients undergoing surgery for renal calculus disease. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 15, 962- 970. Education’s Effects 48 Cassileth, B. R., Hieberger, R. M., March, V., & Sutton-Smith, K. (1982). Effect of audiovisual cancer programs on patients and families. Journal of Medical Education, 57, 54-59. Cochran, T. (1984). Psychological preparation of patients for surgical procedures. Patient Education and Counseling, 5, 153-158. Cuppies, S. A. (1991). Effects of timing and reinforcement of preoperative education on knowledge and recovery of patients having coronary artery bypass graft surgery. Heart & Lung, 20, 654-660. Duryee, R. (1992). The efficacy of inpatient education after myocardial infarction. Heart & Lung, 21, 217-225. Felton, G., Huss, K., Payne, E. A., & Srsic, K. (1976). Preoperative nursing intervention with the patient for surgery: Outcomes of three alternative approaches. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 13, 83-96. Finesilver, C. (1978). Preparation of adult patients for cardiac catheterization and coronary cineangiography. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 15, 211-221. Friedman, S. B., Badere, B„ & Fitzpatrick, S. (1992). The effects of television on preoperative anxiety. Journal of Post. Anesthesia Nursing, 7, 243-250. Fullhart, J. W. (1992). Preparatory information and anxiety before Education’s Effects 49 sigmoidoscopy: A comparative study. Society of Gastroenterology Nurses and Associates, 6, 286-290. Gagliano, M. E. (1988). A literature review on the efficacy of video in patient education. Journal of Medical Education. 63, 785-792. Gerdes, J. E. (1990). An ambulatory approach to outpatient pediatric cardiac catheterization. Journal of Post Anesthesia Nursing, 5, 407-410. Glass, J. C. (1990). Changing death anxiety through death education in the public schools. Death Studies, 14, 31-52. Hagopian, G. A. (1991). The effects of a weekly radiation therapy newsletter on patients. Oncology Nursing Forum, 18, 1199-1203. Hartfield, M. J., Cason, C. L., & Cason, G. J. (1982). Effect of information about a threatening procedure on patients’ expectations and emotional distress. Nursing Research, 31, 202-206. Hartfield, M. J., & Cason, C. L. (1981). Effect of information on emotional responses during barium enema. Nursing Research, 30, 151, 153, 155. Hathaway, D. (1986). Effect of preoperative instruction on postoperative outcomes: A meta-analysis. Nursing Research, 35, 269-275. Hill, D. & Balk, D. (1987). The effect of an education program for families of the chronically mentally ill on stress and anxiety. Psychosocial Education’s Effects 50 Rehabilitation Journal, 10, 25-40. Hill, N. E., Baker, M., Warner, R. A., & Taub, H. (1988). Evaluating the use of a videotape in teaching the pre-cardiac catheterization patient. Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing, 2, 71-78. Kendall, P. C., Williams, L., Pechacek, T. F., Graham, L. E., Shisslak, C., & Herzoff, N. (1979). Cognitive-behavioral and patient education interventions in cardiac catheterization procedures: The Palo Alto medical psychology project. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47, 49-58. Lockard, B. E. (1989). Immediate, residual, and long-term effects of a death education instructional unit on the death anxiety level of nursing students. Death Studies, 13, 137-159. Miller, T. W. (1981). Life events scaling: Clinical methodological issues. Nursing Research, 30, 316-320. Mishel, M. H. (1984). Perceived uncertainty and stress in illness. Research in Nursing and Health, 7, 163-171. Oberle, K., Wry, J., Paul, P., & Grace, M. (1990). Environment, anxiety, and postoperative pain. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 12, 745-757. Pappenheim, C. L., & Kirkpatrick, B. (1988). Cardiac catheterization. Education’s Effects 51 Performing the procedure in an outpatient setting. The Association of Operating Room Nurses Journal, 48, 1130-1137. Peace, H. G., & Vincent, P. A. (1988). Death anxiety: Does education make a difference? Death Studies, 12, 337-344. Peterson, M. (1991). Patient anxiety before cardiac catheterization: an intervention study. Heart & Lung, 20, 643-647. Robertson,C., Gatchel, R. J., & Fowler, C. (1991). Effectiveness of a videotaped behavioral intervention in reducing anxiety in emergency oral surgery patients. Behavioral Medicine, 17, 77-85. Roy, C. (1984). Introduction to Nursing: An Adaptation Model. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, Inc. Sheridan, K., Humfleet, G., Phair, J., & Lyons, J. (1990). The effects of AIDS education on the knowledge and attitudes of community leaders. Journal of Community Psychology, 18, 354-360. Sigsbee, M., & Geden, E. A. (1990). Effects of anxiety on family members of patients with cardiac disease learning cardiopulmonary resuscitation. Heart & Lung, 19, 662-665. Sime, A. M., & Libera, M. B. (1985). Sensation information, self-instruction and responses to dental surgery. Research in Nursing and Health, 8, 41-47. Education’s Effects 52 Sirles, A. T., Brown, K., & Hilyer, J. C. (1991). Effects of back school education and exercise in back injured municipal workers. American Association of Occupational Health Nurses Journal, 39, 7-12. Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory STAI (Form Y). Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc. Stone, G. L., Wolraich, M. L., & Hillerbrand, E. (1988). Communicating distressful information: A video training program in a special education setting. Journal of Counseling and Development, 66, 438-441. Swindale, J. E. (1989). The nurse’s role in giving pre-operative information to reduce anxiety in patients admitted to hospital for elective minor surgery. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 14, 899-905. Talley, J. D., Joseph, A., & Kupersmith, J. (1990). Outpatient left heart and coronary arteriography: The current practice at the University of Louisville School of Medicine. Kentucky Medical Association Journal, 88, 488-493. Toth, J. C. (1980). Effect of structured preparation for transfer on patient anxiety on leaving coronary care unit. Nursing Research, 29, 28-34. Verderber, A., Shively, M., & Fitzsimmons, L. (1992). Preparation for cardiac catheterization. Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing, 7, 75-77. Education’s Effects 53 Warner, C. D., Peebles, B. U., Miller, J., Reed, R., Rodiquez, S., & MartinLewis, E. (1992). The effectiveness of teaching a relaxation technique to patients undergoing elective cardiac catheterization. Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing, 6, 66-75. Zeidner, M., Klingman, A., & Papko, O. (1988). Enhancing students’ test coping skills: Report of a psychological health education program. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 95-101.