“■TBBB 8 m Developmental Linguistic Realization in Black & White Preschool Children: Local Normative Data by Lisa Petras Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Master of Arts Degree Appp bv: Date ?son, Speech & Communication Studies, Edinboro University of PA Chai JqJjZdvisor fomHiittee Member /&//*/?£, 17 Date LMJAZ O-//?! 9,^ Date AZ/M/9J % Date Committee Member Thesis Sp Com 1992 P493d c.2 Petras, Lisa. Developmental linguistic realization 1992. /'• ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The author wishes to express her gratitude to several persons without whose help and guidance, this research would not have been possible: to my mentor, Charlotte Molrine, for her invaluable editing and statistical input, and also for her consistent support throughout my graduate career; to John Boylan, for his knowledge and expertise in field of phonology and interpretation of the PEPPER program; to Ruth Nash-Thompson, for her personal insights and suggestions about BEV; to the faculty of Edinboro University’s program in Speech-Language Pathology, for enhancing my knowledge and encouraging me to be the student I am today; to Rachelle L Laubach, for her consistent encouragement and support throughout this study; to Keith M. Petras, my brother, who has challenged me and motivated me to succeed; and to my parents, Albert E. Petras and Constance R. Petras, whose belief in my ability has supported and encouraged me to succeed in life. •• li TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ii TABLE OF CONTENTS iii LIST OF TABLES v LIST OF FIGURES vi I. INTRODUCTION 1 II. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE 2 Assessment 2 Characteristics 4 Overview of Research 5 Statement of Problem 10 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY 11 Subjects 11 Examiners 12 Materials 12 Procedures 13 Phonetic Inventory 14 Inter- and Intra-Judge Agreement 15 Statistic Analysis 16 RESULTS AND DISCUSSION 17 Inter- and Intra-Judge Agreement 17 Statistical Analysis 17 Canonical Form 18 III. IV. ni V. Manner Features 21 Place Features 28 Phonological Processes 37 Syllable Structure Processes 38 Subtitution Processes 40 Assimilation Processes 42 CONCLUSION 43 REFERENCES 46 APPENDIX 51 Appendix A : Characteristics of Black English Phonology IV LIST OF TABLES Table 1 Table 2 Table 3 Table 4 Table 5 Table 6 Table 7 Table 8 Table 9 Table 10 Table 11 Three-Way ANOVA for Between-Group Factors of Race and Age and Within-Subject Factor of Canonical Form as Computed by GANOVA-4 19 Three-Way ANOVA for Between-Group Factors of Race and Age and Within-Subject Factor of Nasals as Computed by GANOVA-4 21 Three-Way ANOVA for Between-Group Factors of Race and Age and Within-Subject Factor of Glides as Computed by GANOVA-4 22 Three-Way ANOVA for Between-Group Factors of Race and Age and Within-Subject Factor of Stops as Computed by GANOVA-4 24 Three-Way ANOVA for Between-Group Factors of Race and Age and Within-Subject Factor of Stops as Computed by GANOVA-4 25 Three-Way ANOVA for Between-Group Factors of Race and Age and Within-Subject Factor of Fricatives as Computed by GANOVA-4 28 Three-Way ANOVA for Between-Group Factors of Race and Age and Within-Subject Factor of Labiodentals as Computed by GANOVA-4 31 Three-Way ANOVA for Between-Group Factors of Race and Age and Within-Subject Factor of Alveolars as Computed by GANOVA-4 33 Three-Way ANOVA for Between-Group Factors of Race and Age and Within-Subject Factor of Palatals as Computed by GANOVA-4 35 Three-Way ANOVA for Between-Group Factors of Race and Age and Within-Subject Factor of Velars as Computed by GANOVA-4 36 Children Evidencing Unstressed Syllable Deletion Process (USD) 38 v Table 12 Children Evidencing Final Consonant Deletion Process (FV) 39 Children Evidencing Cluster Reduction Process (CR) 40 Children Evidencing Liquid Simplification Process(LS) 40 Table 15 Children Evidencing Stopping Process (ST) 41 Table 16 Children Evidencing Velar Fronting Process (VF) 41 Table 17 Children Evidencing Assimilation Process (AS) 42 Table 13 Table 14 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1 Interaction of Age across Canonical Form 20 Figure 2 Interaction of Race Across Fricative Production 26 Figure 3 Interaction of Race Across Labiodental Production 29 Figure 4 Interaction of Race Across Age Groups in Alveolar Production vi 34 Developmental Linguistic Realization in Black & White Preschool Children: Local Normative Data Chapter 1 Review of the Literature Phonological differences between the adult model of Black English (BE) and that of standard English (SE) have been well documented (Dillard, 1972; Labov, 1972; and Wolfram & Fasold, 1974). However, very little has been reported regarding phonological development in Black English speakers. Although speech-language pathology is sensitive to the importance of distinguishing dialectal differences from language disorders, there are very few published guidelines for making a distinction between dialectal difference and phonological processing disorder in the emerging phonology of young Black children. A clinician must compare the phonology of children who use Black English either to the adult model of Black English, or to norms reported for Standard English. Neither comparison seems appropriate for developmental phonological assessment purposes (Haynes & Moran, 1989). For purposes of this discussion, Black English (BE) will be used to denote a dialect used by African Americans in the inner cities of most large urban areas and in the rural South (Owens, 1988). As such, BE should be viewed as a systematic language of its own, with characteristic differences in phonology, morphology, syntax, prosody, 1 pragmatics, and discourse, and not as a deviant or an improper form of Standard English (Cole & Taylor, 1990). Assessment Because ASHA recognizes the legitimacy of all dialects of language (Taylor, 1986), it mandates that speech-language pathologists implement nondiscriminatory testing procedures (Committee on the status of Racial Minorities, 1983; 1987). Nonetheless, one of the most critical problems facing professionals in the field of speech-language pathology is the absence of adequate assessment tools to provide a valid and reliable evaluation of language varieties spoken by nonmainstreamed speakers (Taylor, 1986). Unfortunately, at the present time, no widely accepted standardized techniques exist for assessing the linguistic abilities of children and adults who speak nonstandard dialects (Terrell & Terrell, 1983). In fact, many researchers (Peters, 1979; Reveron, 1984; Seymour & Miller-Jones, 1981; and Vaughn-Cooke, 1980) have demonstrated that most assessment procedures in speechlanguage pathology are culturally- and linguistically-biased towards middle class White Americans and biased against an overwhelming number of Black Americans. In order to differentiate speech-language disorders from speech-language differences, such as might be exhibited by non-mainstreamed speakers, and to avoid systematic discrimination practices that often result in a high proportion of African-American children being diagnosed as speech or language impaired (Cole & Taylor, 1990), accurate assessment procedures must be developed. Various scholars and practitioners have proposed alternate procedures to assess speech and language functions in nonmainstreamed speakers. These include: (1) standardizing existing tests on non-mainstreamed English speakers; (2) one including a small percentage of minorities in the standardization sample when developing a test; (3) one utilizing a language sample when assessing the language of non-mainstreamed speakers; (4) utilizing criterionreferenced measures when assessing the language of nonmainstreamed speakers; (5) refraining from using all standardized test that have not been corrected for test bias when assessing the language of non-mainstream speakers; (6) developing a new test which can provide a more appropriate assessment of the language of nonmainstreamed English speakers; and (7) modifying or revising existing tests in ways that will make them appropriate for non-mainstreamed speakers, (Boyd & Vaughn-Cooke, 1983). As each of these alternatives have inherent strengths and weakness, the reader is referred to Vaughn-Cooke (1983) for a more thorough discussion. For the AfricanAmerican population, however, the most common assessment alternatives proposed by the assessment researchers are modifying or revising existing tests, utilizing criterion-referenced measures, and developing new tests. Characteristics of Black English In order to modify, revise, or develop new tests, the characteristics of the dialect in question must be examined. Since the focus of this research is the development of linguistic realization in African American children, the discussion of Black English will be confined to the features of the sound system. Black English exhibits two types of sound system variations: a variation within itself, from region to region; and a variation between its sound system and that of Standard English. Like standard English with its regional variable rules for the presence versus absence of /r/ in /’-less types words (i.e., /bar/ versus /bah/), Black English also has its own regional variables rules, such as the vocalic lowering /fa/ to £•/ in the South for words like cheer and steer. Black English also displays a second type of variation. Depending upon the perceived formality of the context and/or audience, African-Americans may pronounce forms more like those of Standard English, or less like those of standard English. Black English, like other dialects then, is doubly varied, both within itself, and in its closeness to Standard English (Moulton, 1976). Extensive sociolinguistic research (Moulton,!976; Owens, 1988; and Wolfram & Fasold, 1974) into the major phonological structures of BEV has resulted in an inventory of potentially different phonological processes in the BE speaker. The reader is referred to Appendix A for an annotated inventory of the BE phonological processes and the rules governing their phonetic realization. Although this type of research illuminates the variable differences in phoneme production of adult BE speakers from adult Standard English speakers, it does not adequately describe the developmental sequence of sound acquisition in children exposed to Black English. To this end, Haynes and Moran (1989) researched final consonant production in southern Black children from preschool through third grade. Using the sound-in-words subtest of the Goldman Fristoe Test of Articulation Revised (GFTA-R) (Goldman & Fristoe, 1986) data was collected on the responses of 222 Black children in rural east central Alabama who used the Black English dialect common to that region. The children's thirteen phonological processes were analyzed using the PROPH computer program. Overall, the data suggested that southern Black children continued to delete final consonants well beyond the age indicated by norms gathered on predominantly White subjects. Both Grunwell (1982) and Ingram (1976) suggested that the final consonant deletion process should be eliminated by age three, in the largely White, standard English speaking population that was studied. Given the contrast between the results of the two studies, Haynes and Moran speculated that Black children appeared to be on a different developmental timetable for the acquisition of final consonant phonemes, as final consonant deletion continued during the preschool and kindergarten years. Indeed, they hypothesized that deletion of final consonants might persist beyond the age of five with certain types of consonants, specifically voiced stops and some fricatives being more susceptible to deletion. Although the researchers speculated that omission of stops might be related to later final consonant deletion and final consonant devoicing rules of the adult model of Black English (i.e., /p/ for /b/f /t/ for /d/, and /k/ for /g/), the deletion of fricatives /v/ and /•#•/ was more difficult to interpret, given the lack of developmental data available for such insights. Because it was of dubious value to compare Black English to norms generated on White, middle-class children, and because neither the existing norms, nor the adult model of Black English, described this development, Haynes and Moran (1989) suggested that clinicians needed to generate local norms for phonological process occurrence and to compare children to these data as appropriate (Cole, 1983). Haelsig and Madison (1986) completed research on the phonological processes exhibited by three, four, and five-year-oldchildren of unspecified race. The subjects were fifty English speaking children, 25 males and 25 females, ranging in age from 2-10 to 5-2. All subjects were obtained from preschools and daycare centers. The Phonological Process Analysis (PPA) (Weiner, 1979) was administered to all subjects as specified in the manual. The PPA elicited 144 responses from which 16 phonological processes were examined. The researchers found that although three-year-old children exhibited errors involving three of 16 processes, 15 of the 16 processes were present across this age group although, the distribution was uneven. Ouster reduction was exhibited by all children; eight of the 10 subjects demonstrated gliding of liquids, weak syllable deletion, glottal replacement, labial assimilation, vocalization, and deletion of final consonants. In contrast, final consonant devoicing, denasalization, velar assimilation, prevocalic voicing, and gliding of fricatives occurred in three or fewer subjects; affrication did not occur at all in three year old subjects. Labial assimilation typically involved the substitution of /•£/ for /£/ as in /Pnrfl/ for thumb. The greatest reduction in the use of phonological processes occurred between the three and four year olds. Deletion of final consonants, stopping, fronting, and gliding of liquids were processes whose frequency was reduced by 50% in the four year old subjects, aiding of liquids was also the most frequently occurring process in four year olds, but no longer occurred in the five year old group. Weak syllable deletion, by contrast, was a frequently exhibited process in all age groups. Glottal replacement of phonemes was infrequently used by four and five year old children. Labial assimilation and cluster reduction showed a similar pattern. Use of vocalization, stopping and alveolar assimilation decreased systematically in the four and five year old age groups. From these findings Haelsig and Madison (1986) suggested that those evaluating phonology should remember that there is a great deal of individual variation in the three year old age group. Phonological acquisition, whether defined in terms of individual phonemes or their realization rules, varies from child to child. Cole and Taylor (1990) studied the extent to which phonological performance of a group of African-American children varied as a function of test-client congruence on three tests of articulation containing Standard English (SE) assumptions; and the extent to which the children's test performance was likely to be misdiagnosed as being pathological when dialect considerations were not taken into account. Ten African-American children, five males and five females, ranging in age from 5-11 to 6-11, served as subjects for this study. The subjects were first graders enrolled in a public school in Lafayette County, Mississippi. They were from a lower socioeconomic status as determined by the schools' free lunch standards. The picture test of the Templin-Darley Tests of Articulation Second Edition (Templin & Darley 1969), the Arizona Articulation Proficiency Scale: Revised (Fudula, 1974), and the Photo Articulation Test (Pendergast, Dicky, Selman, & Sorder, 1969) were administered to each subject individually. Two scores were assigned to each response. One score was assigned from live elicitations in accordance with the rules of Standard English or the stated criteria of each test. The second score was based on judgments from both the live elicitations and the audiotapes of the live elicitations for all items on which phonological rules of BEV could be appropriately employed by the subjects. The results showed the mean raw score obtained by the subjects on the Arizona Articulation Proficiency Scale using SE norms was 90.9, while the mean raw score was 96.95 using BEV rules. According to the norms of standard English, seven subjects met the test's criteria for an articulation disorder, however, none of these same subjects was considered as having an articulation disorder when BEV criteria was employed. Similarly, the mean score obtained by the subjects on the Templin Dariey Test of Articulation using standard English norms was 125.2; however, the mean score increased to 132.6 when BEV rules were employed. Six of the children were diagnosed as having an articulation disorder when SE norms were used, but this number was reduced to two when BEV rules were applied. Finally, the mean number of errors obtained by the subjects on the Photo Articulation Test was reduced from 5.2 using SE norms to 2.3 using BEV rules, and the number of children meeting the test's criterion for articulation disorders was reduced from three to one when BEV scoring criteria was employed. In general, these results corroborate Cole's (1986) claim that African-American children perform differently on standardized tests of articulation as a function of the linguistic norms used to score items. Moreover, the results clearly showed that a failure to take the issue of dialect variation into account substantially increases the likelihood of misdiagnosing normally speaking African-American children as having articulation disorders. Statement of Problem As the preceding research has shown, little normative data has been published about the developmental phonology of Black English speaking children, and the normative data that has been reported has come from southern communities. Most descriptive studies of Black English have focused on adults, adolescents and children who have acquired the adult system. The few empirical studies of young Black children's speech have drawn upon limited phonological samples, and have isolated only a few features of Black English for investigation (Seymour & Seymour, 1981). The purpose of the present study is to fill the gap in the normative literature on the development of sound acquisition and phonological processing patterns in White and African-American five and six-year old children matched for socioeconomic status. Independent variables for the study are race, age, and phonetic feature, phonological process, or canonical form. Given the present trend in the research reviewed to date, two primary hypotheses were formulated: 1) that five- and six-year old Black children would perform significantly different than their White peers with respect to speech production parameters; and 2) that fiveand six-year old Black children would evidence a significantly different timetable of development with respect to speech production parameters. Chapter II Method Subjects From a pool of 67 preschool children attending the Crawford County Head Start Program in Meadville, Pennsylvania, 56 Black and White children, ranging in age from five to six years served as subjects for this study. Four 4-year olds were eliminated from the pool, as their number was not felt to be sufficient to make constructive inferences about their phonological development. One five year old was eliminated also from the subject pool due to the fact that he could not verbalize his receptive knowledge of the pictures used to elicit phonemes for the study. Additionally 6 children, ranging in age from 5 to 6 years, who were receiving speech and/or language services at the Head Start facility, were were eliminated from the subject pool. All children serving as subjects ranged in age from 60 to 83 months, and were from low socioeconomic backgrounds, as classified by the associate director of the Head Start Program. All children resided in the industrial community of Meadville, Pennsylvania, located in the Great Lakes' region of northwestern Pennsylvania. The Meadville community represents a mixture of ethnic groups, predominantly of Irish, Italian, and Polish decent, and more recently of Hispanic and African-American heritage. Examiners In addition to the researcher, five graduate student speechlanguage clinicians from Edinboro University of Pennsylvania served as examiners for this study. Examiners were White, Standard English speakers, with experience in articulation test administration. All examiners were supervised at the Head Start site by a licensed and certified speech-language pathologist employed by Edinboro University of Pennsylvania. Materials The sound-in-words subtest of the Goldman Fristoe Test of Articulation Revised{GFTA-R) (Goldman & Fristoe, 1986) was used in the study to elicit word-level production of 44 consonantal phonemes. The GFTA-R was chosen for its ease of administration, its attention to individual sound production, and its compatibility with phonological process analysis protocols. The GFTA-R was normed on thirty-seven articulatoiy defective children between the ages of four and eight years. No information on the race of the children was given in the manual. The sounds-in-words subtest consists of 35 pictures depicting objects and activities that are familiar to young children. The subject is required to name the pictures and to reply to questions about nine of them, for a total of 44 responses. The 44 responses elicit consonantal phonemes in each of three word positions: initial, medial, and final. The Programs to Examine Phonetic and Phonological Evaluation Records (PEPPER) (Shriberg, 1986) was used to analyze all responses. The PEPPER program examines distinctive features and phonological process patterns characteristic of speech responses, be they syllables, words, or strings of words, for the age groups of concern in this study. Procedures Each child came to one of four examination rooms with a "postit" sticker identifying his/her date of birth. If the child was participating in facility-sponsored speech and/or language therapy, the addition of a star accompanied the birthdate. Each clinician recorded the child's date of birth, and the star designation, if appropriate. Each child was given the sound-in-words subtest of the GFTA-R. The responses elicited from each subject were broadly phonetically transcribed at the time of administration by each of the five examiners, with audiotape back-up of each test administration. The tape recordings were to be used later to verify broad transcription, add narrow transcription, and to compute inter-judge and intra-judge reliability estimates. After verifying broad transcription, individual test sheets were narrowly transcribed by the researcher using the narrow transcription protocol of Shriberg and Kent (1982). Questionable narrow transcription analysis was resolved through consultation with Mr. John Boylan, CCC-SLP, a thesis committee member with expertise in phonology. Phonetic Inventory Once narrow transcription of each subjects responses were complete, the following subject groupings were formed: Group A1 White five-year -olds (N = 25); Group B1 - Black five-year olds (N = 12); Group A2 - White six-year olds (N =14); and Group B2 - Black six-year olds (N = 5) These data was entered into the PEPPER computer program. Due to the PEPPER program's processing limitations of 12 subject entries per analysis, the groups were subdivided as follows. The Black and White five-year-olds were analyzed in four month sequences; such that Group A1 consisted of the following White fiveyear-old subgroups: Ala - ages 5-0 to 5-3 (N = 7); Alb - ages 5-4 to 5-8 (N = 10); and Ale - ages 5-9 to 5-12 (N = 8); similarly, Group B1 consisted of Black five-year-old subgroups: Bla - ages 5-0 to 5-3 (N = 6); Bib - ages 5-4 to 5-8 (N = 4); and Blc - ages 5-9 to 5-12 (N = 4). Six-year-old White and six-year-old Black children were analyzed as separate individual groups individual groups since the number of subjects in each group did not exceed the 12 entry limitations of PEPPER Specifically, there were 12 children in the white six-year-old group and 5 children in the Black six-year-old group. For each of the 56 subject responses entered, PEPPER performed the following analyses: maintenance of canonical forms; phonetic analysis by manner and place feature; and classification of the percentage of deletions, substitutions, and distortions of phonemes in each of the three positions; and usage of phonological process forms. The percentage of accuracy the individual achieved with each of the forms and features analyzed was generated by PEPPER Inter- and Intra-Judge Agreement Following the initial data collection period, the researcher listened to the test administration recordings of four randomly selected subjects from her test administration group and broadly retranscribed the GFTA-R's sound-in words responses to establish intra-judge agreement. Point-by-point percentage of agreement for English phonetic transcription of the 44 elicited word responses were computed, using the transcription reliability formula recommended by Brenneise, Sarshad, Nicholas, and Brookshire (1991). (Agreed-upon words/total words transcribed by examiner) X100. For all 202 phonetic elements, point-by-point percent agreement reliability was calculated for each phoneme using the following formula: [Total agreements/(Total agreements + Total disagreements)] X100. Inter-judge reliability between the researcher and each of the five other speech-language clinicians was established from four randomly selected audiotaped test administrations utilizing the aforementioned formulas. Statistical Analyses A series of 3-way analyses of variance (ANOVA) with fixed effects were computed by the GANOVA-4 (Brecht, Woodward & Bonett, 1986) computer program using a two-between-group onewithin-subject mixed design with alpha set at .05. The between-group factors were always race (Factor A), with two levels, white subjects and Black subjects, and age (Factor B), with four levels; children 5-0 to 5-3 years of age, 5-4 to 5-8 years of age, 5-5 to 5-12 years of age, and 6-0 to 6-12 years of age. The within-subject factor (Factor C) varied with manner features (nasals, 3 levels; glides, 2 levels; stops, 6 levels; fricatives, 6 levels; affricatives, 2 levels; and liquids, 2 levels), place features (bilabials, labiodentals, dentals, alveolars, palatals, and velars, all having 3 levels for each sound position [initial, medial, and final], and canonical form (6 levels). Reflecting the population sample under study, the design was unbalanced with more total White subjects (N=37) than Black subjects (N=19). Chapter III Results and Discussion Inter- and Intra-Judge Agreement Point-by-point percentage of agreement for English phonetic transcription of the 44 elicited word responses by the researcher was 96.6%, and for all 202 phonetic elements, 99.25%. For each of the student examiners, point-by-point percentage of agreement for the 44 elicited word responses ranged from 81.3% agreement to 99.5% agreement, with a mean percentage of agreement of 91.94%. The one examiner with the lowest percentage of transcription reliability (81.3%) consistently used a incorrect vowel phoneme in her transcription; specifically she transcribed / i / for /X / and j&J for /£ /. Similarly, point-by-point percent of agreement for the 202 phonetic elements transcribed by the student examiners ranged from 95.9% agreement to 98.6% agreement, with a mean percentage of agreement of 97.1%. Statistical Analysis Because compound symmetry was found to be significant (p <.001) with all 3-way ANOVAs, the Wilks' Lamda (R) value, an alternative multivariate test computed by GANOVA-4, was utilized for all within-subject factors to determine the probability of significance. Only those main effects and interactions found to be significant at the .05 level of significance were further analyzed. 17 The Scheffe' Test formula for unbalanced designs was used for post-hoc testing within-subject factors and between-within subject interactions because of the lack of compound symmetry. The Neuman-Keuls Test formula for unbalanced designs was used for posthoc testing the between portion and between group interactions of the design (Maxwell & Bray, 1986). An estimate of was computed for for the three-way interactions to determine the percentage of variance accounted for by the interaction All three-way interactions were found to have a^f of 5% or less, and were not pursued with further analysis. The results of significant main effects and first order interactions follow. Canonical Form Table 1 presents the results of the three-way mixed AN OVA for inbetween-groups factors of race and age, and within-subject factor of canonical form, as computed by GANOVA-4. A main effect for Factor Q canonical form, was found at the .006 level of significance (Wilks' Lamda (R) = 3.844; df = 5, 44). Post-hoc analysis of the differences between the canonical forms revealed that all children, independent of race or age, evidenced significantly better performance with CVC forms (99%) than with CnV forms (87.4%). Similarly, a significant interaction of age and canonical form was found at the .007 level of significance (F = 2.300; df = 15,121). Post-hoc analysis of the age factor across canonical forms indicated that children between the ages of of 5-9 and 5-12, independent of race, evidenced significant difficulty acquiring CnV (78.1%) and CnVC (76.8%) forms, whereas children between the ages of 6-0 to 6-12, independent of race, were having significant difficulty acquiring CnV (83.8%) and 3+ syllable (87.9%) forms (see Figure 1). Table 1 Three-Way ANOVA for Retween-Croup Factors of Race and Age and Within-Subject Factor of Canonical Form as Computed by GANOVA-4 Univariate Tests F OF £ Main effect Factor A (race) 0.062 1,48 >.500 Main Effect Factor B (age) 0.447 3,48 >.500 Interaction of A and B 0.732 3,48 >.500 Alternate Multivariate Tests Wilks' DF Lambda (R) P Main Effect Factor C (canonical form) 3.844 5,44 .006** Interaction of A and C 1.130 5,44 .359 Interaction of B and C 2.300 15,121 .007** Interaction of ABC 2,778 15,121 .001** *p< .05. **p< .01 h Note. For interaction ABC= -04 Figure 1. Interaction of Age x Canonical Form 80% 75% la lb lc 2a Note, o = CVC; x = CnV; U = CnVQ * = c(n)VCn; + = 2-syllable; and o = 3+ syllable la = ages 5-0 to 5-3; ages lb = 5-4 to 5-8; ages lc = 5-9 lo 5-12; 2a age 6-2 to 6-12 The developmental literature suggests that the processes of cluster reduction and final consonant deletion contribute to a high proportion of CVC syllable shapes (Stoel-Gammon & Dunn, 1985). Indeed, as the phonological process analysis reveals the children studied in this sample persist in their usage of Final Consonant Deletion (especially the White children) and Ouster Reduction. The sample data also indicates that the difficulty with cluster reduction is most prevalent in the initial position, with consonant sequence reduction occurring in utterances such as /tov/ for /stov/, and /dum/ for /drum/ (Hodson & Paden, 1991) Manner Features Significant main effects for the following manner features were found: nasals, glides, stops, and fricatives. No significant main effects or interactions were found for liquids or affricatives, indicating that neither races nor age groups differed significantly in their productions of liquids or affricative. Table 2 Three-way mixed A NOVA for Retween-Group Factors of Race and Age and Within Suhject Factor of Nasals as Computed by HA NOVA-4 Univariate Tests F DF P Main Effect Factor A (race) 0.903 1,48 .349 Main Effect Factor B (age) 0.794 3,48 >.500 Interaction of A and B 2.643 3,48 .059 Alternate Mutivariate Tests Wilks' Lamda (R) DF P Main Effect Factor C (nasals) 39.054 2,47 <.001** Interaction of A and C 0.536 2,47 >.500 Interaction of B and C 0.871 6,94 >.500 Interaction of ABC 1.125 6,94 .354 *p = <.05; ** p = <.01 For the significant main effect of Factor C, nasals (/m/, /n/, and )Y\ /), all the children sampled, independent of race or age, had a 3 significantly poorer performance with the phoneme /^ / (66.7%) than with either /m/ (97.9%) or /n/ (99.1%). These data are consistent with the developmental literature that reports that front nasals /m/ and /n/ are usually among the first sounds to be learned by Standard English speaking children. The velar nasal /fj / is learned somewhat later. It is frequently replaced by { n ), as in [swin] for swing or [win] for ring (Edwards & Shriberg, 1983; Moulton, 1986). Table 3 Three-way ANOVA for Between-Group Factors of Race and Age, and Within Subject Factor of Glides as computed by CANOVA-4 (Univariate Tests) F DF P Main Effect Factor A (race) 0.219 1,48 >.500 Main Effect Factor B (age) 0.318 3,48 >.500 Interaction of A and B 2.615 3,48 .061 (Alternate Mutivariate Tests) Wilks' Lam da (R) DF P Main Effect Factor C (glides) 16.520 1,48 <.001** Interaction of A and C 0.000 1,48 >.500 Interaction of B and C 0.189 3,48 >.500 Interaction of ABC 1.163 3,48 .333 *p = <.05; ** p = <.01 For the significant main effect of Factor C, glides (/w/, /j/) all the children sampled, independent of race or age, had a significantly poorer performance with the phoneme /j/ (76.8%) then the phoneme /w/ (95%.6). Edwards & Shriberg (1983) found that, in general, glides tend to be replaced by liquids, as in the example / realized as /1 &|o/, or front glides /w/ tends to replace the palatalized /j/ , as in the example /jelo / realized as /u>elo/. They have found also that /r/ and /!/ have stable positions during production, but that there tends to be movement (i.e. gliding) from one articulatory position to another during the production of /w/ and /j/. Edwards & Shriberg noted that, in general, sounds produced in the front of the mouth are acquired earlier than sounds produced in the back of the mouth. Before back sounds are learned, they may be replaced by similar front sounds. This was noted by the researcher in a frequent substitution of /w/ for /j/ in transcriptions of children's responses. A significant main effect for Factor C, stops, was found at p< .001 level of significance (Wilks' Lamda = 4.681; df = 5, 240). Post-hoc analysis of the differences between stops cognates (/p-b/, /t-d/, and /kg/) revealed that all children sampled, independent of race or age, had no significant difficulty producing stops, irregardless of voicing or place of production features. Accuracy with all stops analyzed was found to be greater than 90%. In order of production proficiency the following trend was found: /b/ (99.6%); /d/ (97.9%); /p/ (96.3%); /g/ (94.5%); ft/ (91.9%); and /k/ (91.4%). Table 4 Ihr6e-way ANOVA for Between-Group Factors of Race and Age, and Within Subject Factor of Stops as Univariate Tests F DF P Main Effect Factor A (race) 0.050 1,48 >.500 Main Effect Factor B (age) 0.175 3,48 >.500 Interaction of A and B 1.453 3,48 .238 Alternate Mutivariate Tests Wilks' Lamda (R) DF P Main Effect Factor C (stops) 6.783 5,44 <.001** Interaction of A and C 1.807 5,44 .131 Interaction of B and C 1.284 15,121 >.500 Interaction of ABC 1.222 15,121 .264 *p = <.05; ** p = <.01 As expected developmental data supported these findings in that stops have been found to be acquired early. Macken (1980) reported that there was no universal or invariant order of acquisition of stops, although there were general patterns. In contrast, Ferguson (1975) proposed an order of stop acquisition, from most to least stable: t > k > b > d > p > g. In the present study, a different stop acquisition pattern was revealed, from most to least stable: b>d>p>g>t>k, with the children in the study having more difficulty with the /t/ and /k/ phonemes. Table 5 Three-way ANOVA for Between-Group Factors of Race and Age and Within Subject Factor of Fricatives as Univariate Tests F DF P Main Effect Factor A (race) 1.161 1,48 .287 Main Effect Factor B (age) 0.293 3,48 >.500 Interaction of A and B 1.772 3,48 .164 Alternate Mutivariate Tests Wilks' Lamda (R) DF P Main Effect Factor C (fricatives) 17.438 5,44 <.001** Interaction of A and C 2.594 5,44 .038* Interaction of B and C 1.361 15,121 .177 Interaction of ABC 2.056 15,121 .016* *p = <.05; ** p = <.01 Note. ~y *= .04 for ABC Interaction A significant main effect of Factor C, fricatives ( /f/, /v/, /O/, / &/, /s/, and /z/) (Wilks' Lamda (R) = 17.438; df = 5, 44; p< .001), indicated that all children sampled, independent of race or age, had significantly more difficulty with the /v/ (76%) than the / f/ (97%) phoneme, but a significantly better performance with the /f-v/ (86.8%) and /s-z/ (89.1%) cognate pairs than with the /&- %/ (60.1%) cognate pairs. Similarly, a significant interaction of race and fricatives was found at the .04 level of significance (Wilks' Lamda = 2.594; df = 5,44). Post-hoc analysis of the race across fricative production (see Figure 2) indicated that only the production of the phoneme /0*/ by Blacks (43.5%) was significantly different from that of Whites (63.5%). Figure Z Interaction of Race Across Fricative Production 100 95 90 85 80 75 70 65 60 55 50 45 40 lol / 0/ = Black Children Note. X = White Children; O If/ /v/ Acquisition of fricatives and affricatives is more complicated than the acquisition of stops. Like stops, fricatives are acquired gradually, and correct production often fluctuates with various types of substitutes (Edwards, 1979; and Ferguson, 1975; ). There is no universal order of acquisition for fricatives, but in general, front fricatives are said to acquired before back fricatives. An exception is /©?, which is acquired late. Ingram, Christensen, Veach, and Webster (1980) found the following order of acquisition, based on percentage of correct production, across 73 subjects: /f/, /s/, /v/, /z/, /&/. Similarly, the present study found the same acquisition of fricatives for both Black and White children, with the exception of / $y occurring before /-O'/, which was not looked at in the Ingram et al. study. Ferguson (1975) also noted that before some fricatives are acquired, they may be replaced by sounds of tighter closure (stops and affricatives). Fricatives may also be omitted, especially in clusters in the word-final position. Moulton (1976) stated that in Black English Vernacular the voiced "th" (/#/) becomes /d/ in the initial position, so that then and den sound alike. Medially, the voiced "th" (/#7)is realized as /d/ or /v/, so that other and udder or either and eva sound alike. Similarly the voiceless "th" (/$•/) in the initial position is realized as /f /or /v/, so that think becomes fink or vink. Medially the voiceless "th" ( /& /) is realized as /f/, making toothbrush into toofbrush. In the final position, the voiceless "th" (/ft/) changes to /f/ or /t/, making fourth to fort. Similarly, the present studied reveal that both Black and White children are substituting stops /t/ and /d/ for fricatives /$/ and /$ /, as Moulton found. Place Features Significant main effects for the following place features were found: labio-dentals, dentals, alveiors, palatals, and velars. No significant main effect or interactions were found for bilabials in any word position, indicating that neither race nor age group differed significantly in their production of bilabial phonemes. Table 6 Three-way ANOVA for R etween - Crou p Factors of Race and Age and Within Subject Factor of Iabio-Dentals as computed by GANOVA-4 Univariate Tests F DF P Main Effect Factor A (race) 1.016 1,48 .320 Main Effect Factor B (age) 0.694 3,48 >.500 Interaction of A and B 2.019 3,48 .123 Alternate Mutivariate Tests Wilks' . Lamda (R) DF P Main Effect Factor C (labio-dentals) 9.764 2,47 <.001 Interaction of A and C 4.522 2,47 .016* Interaction of B and C 2.176 6,94 .051 Interaction of ABC 2.527 6,94 .026* *p = <.05; ** p = <-01 Note.'ty x= .04 for ABC Interaction The significant main effect for Factor C, labio-dentals, was found at p< -001 level of significance (Wilks' Lamda (R) = 97.6; df = 2, 47). Post-hoc testing revealed that all the children sampled, independent of race or age, had significantly poorer performance on labiodentals in the medial position (80.4%) than in either the initial (93.5%) or final (92.4%) positions. Similarly, a significant interaction of race and labiodental production was found at the .02 level of significance (Wilks' Lamda (R) = 4.522; df = 2,47). Post-hoc analysis of the race across labiodental production (see Figure 3) indicated that Blacks (72.3%) evidenced a significantly different performance from Whites (88.6%) on labiodentals in the medial position. Figure 3 Interaction of Race Across Labiodental Production 100 95 90 85 80 75 70 initial position Note. medial position X = White Children; O = Black Children final position Developmental data of sound positions from Edwards and Shriberg (1983) indicate that, in general, sounds produced in the front of the mouth are acquired earlier than sounds produced in the back of the mouth. Before back sounds are learned, they may be replaced by similar front sounds. It was stated that sounds are acquired first in the initial position, then medial position, and then final. However, while in the initial position does seem to favored stops, there is evidence that fricatives may be acquired first in the postvocalic or final position. The results of this study indicated that Black and White child have acquired acquisition of labiodentals in the initial and final positions, but are still having difficulty with the medial position. Analysis of the data collected for this study found a significant substitution of bilabials (/p/ and /b/) for labiodental sounds (/f / and /v/) in the medial position. For example, children realized [telepone] for [telephone], thus substituting the /p/ for /f/; and realized [shobel] for [shovel], thus substituting the /b/ for /v/. Therefore, the present data indicated that more of the Black children sampled are simplifying and/or substituting labiodental fricatives in the medial position with bilabial stops. The significant main effect for Factor C, dentals, was found at the .004 level of significance (Wilks' Lamda (R) = 6.381; df = 2, 47). Post-hoc analysis revealed that, independent of race or age, all children sampled had significantly more difficulty with dentals in the final position (512%) as compared to the initial position (67.5%). No significant difference was found with dentals in the medial (53.8%) as compared with the final position (51.2%). Table 7 Three-way ANOVA for Between-Croup Factors of Race and Ager and Within Subject Factor of Dentals as computed hv CIA NOVA-4 (Univariate Tests) F DF P Main Effect Factor A (race) 3.902 1,48 .056 Main Effect Factor B (age) 1.082 3,48 .366 Interaction of A and B 1.302 3,48 .284 (Alternate Mutivariate Tests) Wilks' Lamda (R) DF P Main Effect Factor C (dentals) 6.381 2,47 <.004** Interaction of A and C 1.614 2,47 .208 Interaction of B and C 1.666 6,94 .137 Interaction of ABC 1.567 6,94 .165 *p = <.05; ** p = <.01 Mouton (1976) found that the dental fricatives / $/ and /©*/ are frequently replaced by Black children with the corresponding dental stops /d/ and /t/. For example [thick] is pronounced as [tick! and [this] is pronounced as [dis], or a dental fricative is replaced with the corresponding voiced labial fricative /v/, so that [brother] is pronounced as [bruwer]. However, is is noted that the replacement of /e/ by /t/ or /f/, and the replacement of /#/ by /d/ or /v/, are by no means unique to Black English. These substitutions are common among nonstandard white speakers. The present study found the same substitutions, with a higher percentage of /*0/ substitutions of /t/ or / f/, as in [bath] and [bathtub] for both Black and White children, possibly accounting for the apparent difficulty evidenced with final and medial sound positions. The significant main effect of Factor C, alveolars (Wilks' Lamda (R) = 6.858, df = 2, 47; /7= .003), found all children sampled, independent of age or race, to have a significantly more accurate performance with alveolars in the final position (95.9%) than in either the initial (90.9%) or medial (91%) positions, as revealed by the post-hoc analysis. A race by age interaction was found to be significant at the .04 level of significance (F = 3.073; df = 3, 48). Post-hoc analysis indicated that Blacks performed differently than Whites as a function of age (see Figure 4). Specifically, Blacks evidenced significantly better performance with alveolar productions (95.9%) than whites (85.6%) at 5-0 to 5-3 years of age and again at 5-9 to 5-12 years of age (Blacks = 99.5%; Whites = 86.3%). The performance trend, however, was dramatically reversed in the 6-0 to 6-12 age group, with Whites (96.8%) evidencing significantly better performance than Black (85.6%) with alveolar productions. An examination of error patterns exhibited by Black Children in the 6-9 to 6-12 age group revealed that the majority of their errors were one of substitution involving a velar phoneme (44%). Table 8 Three-wav ANQVA for Between-Croup Factors of Race and Age and Within Subject Factor of Alveolars as computed hv GANOVA-4 Univariate Tests F DF P Main Effect Factor A (race) 0.864 1,48 .360 Main Effect Factor B (age) 0.147 3,48 >.500 Interaction of A and B 3.073 3,48 .036* Alternate Mutivariate Tests Wilks' Lamda (R) DF P Main Effect Factor C (alveolars) 6.858 2,47 <.003** Interaction of A and C 0.525 2,47 >.500 Interaction of B and C 1.542 6,94 .172 Interaction of ABC 3.298 6,94 .006** *p = <.05; ** p = <01 Note. = .05 for ABC Interaction Figure 4 Interaction of Race across Age Groups 100 96 92 88 84 80 76 72 la lb lc 2a Note. X = White children; O = Black children; la = ages 5-0 to 5-3; lb = ages 5-4 to 5-8; lc ages 5-9 to 5-12; 2a = ages 6.2 to 6-12. The main effect for Factor C, palatals, was found at p< .001 level of significance (Wilks' Lamda = 8.566; df = 2, 47). Post-hoc analysis revealed that all children sampled, independent of age or race, had significantly better performance within palatals in the final position (94.97e) than in either the initial (86.47.0 or medial (84.6%) positions. Table 9 Three-way ANOVA for Between-Group Factors of Race and Age and -Within Subject Factor of Palatals as computed hv GANOVA-4 Univariate Tests F DF P Main Effect Factor A (race) 1.849 1,48 .177 Main Effect Factor B (age) 1.057 3,48 .377 Interaction of A and B 0.950 3,48 .426 'Alternate Mutivariate Tests Wilks' Lamda (R) DF P Main Effect Factor C (palatals) 8.566 2,47 <.001** Interaction of A and C 1.922 2,47 .156 Interaction of B and C 1,722 6,94 .124 Interaction of ABC 0.729 6,94 >.500 *p = <.05; ** p = <.01 Similarly, the main effect for Factor C, velars, was found at p< .001 level of significance (Wilks' Lamda (R) = 21.271; df = 2,47). Post hoc analysis revealed that all children sampled, independent of race or age, had significantly more difficulty producing velars in the final position (73.7%) as compared to either the medial (89.7%) or initial (92.2%)positions. Table 10 Three-wav ANOVA for Between-Group Factors nf Race and Ager and Within Subject Factor nf Velars as computed by nANOVA-4 Univariate Tests F DF P Main Effect Factor A (race) 0.043 1,48 >.005 Main Effect Factor B (age) 0.208 3,48 >.500 Interaction of A and B 0.916 3,48 .443 Alternate Mutivariate Tests Wilks' Lamda (R) DF P Main Effect Factor C (dentals) 21.271 2,47 <.001** Interaction of A and C 1.378 2,47 .261 Interaction of B and C 0.466 6,94 >.500 Interaction of ABC 1.363 6,94 .237 *p = <.05; ** p = <.01 For both palatal and velar processes these data are consistent with developmental data. In terms of the place of articulation, the most common pattern of acquisition is front consonant before back one: labial > alveolars > palatal > velar. Consonants acquired often substitute for those learned later, e.g., alveolars for palatals (depalatalization) (Stoel-Gammon & Dunn, 1985). The present study revealed substitution patterns by both Black and White children of /s/ for lj / as in /sue/ for /shoe/, thus showing depalatalization of alveolar phonemes for palatals. Another error of depalatalization that occurred for both Black and White children was /t/ for /tf /, as in /turch/ for /church/. In terms of velars, depalatization occurred with /n/ for /(j /, thus simplifying a velar sound with an alveolar. Phonological Processes Seven naturally occurring processes were subjectively examined across age and race to identify processes that the sample children were still evidencing. Phonological processes provide a useful framework for describing children's systematic simplifications of the adult phonological systems. Stoel-Gammon and Dunn (1985) group phonological processes into three major categories; (1) processes that modify the syllabic structure of the target word, such as Unstressed Syllable Deletion (USD), Final Consonant Deletion (FCD), and Cluster Reduction (CR); (2) processes that substitute on sound for another, such as Liquid Simplification, Stopping, and Velar Fronting; and (3) processes that assimilate one sound to another, such in Assimilation. AH of these major categories of error patterns were found in the children sampled and will be discussed in the same order as that was suggested by StoelGammon and Dunn. Syllable Structure Processes Unstressed syllable deletion, as the name suggests, is a process that modifies the syllabic structures of the target word through deletion of an unstressed syllable. As presented in Table 11, White children were found to have noticeably more difficulty than their Black peers with the process of unstressed syllable deletion during each of the three earliest age ranges, with the process disappearing entirely in Black children by 5-9 to 5-12. USD, however, appears to be a noticeable factor in the performance of Black children in the later age range (6-9 to 6-12). White children in this study have totally eliminated this process by 6-2 to 6-12, but for Black children in this sample the process reemerges. Table 11 Children Evidencing Unstressed Syllable Deletion Process (USD) White Age Black Children range Number of children using USD Percentage of USD 6/7 86% 6/10 60% 5-9 to 5-12 3/8 6-9 to 6-12 0/12 38% 0% 5-0 to 5-3 5-4 to 5-8 Age Children range 5-0 to 5-3 5-4 to 5-8 Number of children using USD 1/6 1/4 17% 25% 5-9 to 5-12 0/4 0% 6-2 to 6-12 3/5 60% Table 12 presents the pattern of final consonant deletion exhibited by the sampled children. Percentage of USD Table 12 Children exhibiting Final Consonant Deletion fFf.ni White Children Number of Age range children using FCD 5-0 to 5-3 0/7 5-4 to 5-8 2/10 5-9 to 5-12 2/8 6-2 to 6-12 1/12 Black Percentage of FCD 0% 20% 25% 8% Children Number of Age range children using DFC 0/6 5-0 to 5-3 0/4 5-4 to 5-8 0/4 5-9 to 5-12 0/5 6-9 to 6-12 Percentage of FCD Although the percentages for FCD are not significantly large, there does appear to be a consistent pattern of difference between the White and Black children sampled in this study above the age range 5-0 to 5-3. White children consistently showed some percentage of FCD process usage, while Black children consistently demonstrated no usage of this process. The Black childrens' lack of FCD usage may be related to the type of error pattern that Black children are exhibiting in final consonant position; mainly that Black children constantly demonstrate substitution errors, whereas White children here characteristically had more errors of omission (deletion). The last process of syllable modification found in the children studied was Cluster Reduction. As is evident in Table 13. Black children present noticeably less percentage of usage of the process of cluster reduction during the early period of development, than their White peers. Percentage scores tend to level out by age 6-2 to 6-12 with both groups at this stage evidencing this process nearly half of the time. 0% 0% 0% 0% Table 13 Children Exhibiting Clus.ter_Reduction Process (Cm White Children Black Children Number of Percentage Number of of CR Age range children Age range children using CR using CR 57% 4/7 5-0 to 5-3 5-0 to 5-3 1/6 50% 5/10 5-4 to 5-8 5-4 to 5-8 1/4 75% 6/8 5-9 to 5-12 5-9 to 5-12 1/4 42% 5/12 6-2 to 6-12 6-9 to 6-12 2/5 Percentage of CR 17% 25% 25% 40% Substitutions Processes Substitution processes are those sound changes that substitute on e class of sounds for another (Stoel-Gammon & Dunn, 1985). Table 14 presents the error pattern of Liquid Simplification evidenced by the children sampled. This process shows a mixed pattern of usage across races and age groups, White children generally evidencing a higher percentage of usage at all ages, with the exception of ages 5-0 to 5-3. Table 14 Children exhibiting liquid simplification Process (LS) Children Number of Age range children using LS 5-0 to 5-3 1/7 5-4 to 5-8 4/10 5-9 to 5-12 3/8 6-2 to 6-12 5/12 Chi dren Number of Age range children using LS 2/6 5-0 to 5-3 1/4 5-4 to 5-8 0/4 5-9 to 5-12 1/5 6-9 to 6-12 Black White Percentage of LS 14% 40% 38% 42% Table 15 contains the error pattern of stopping evidenced by the Percentage of LS 33% 25% 0% 20% children sampled. Table 15 White Children Age 5-0 5-4 5-9 6-2 range to to to to 5-3 5-8 5-12 6t12 Number of children using ST 1/7 2/10 4/8 1/12 Black Percentage of ST 14% 20% 50% 8% Age 5-0 5-4 5-9 6-9 Chi dren range to to to to 5-3 5-8 5-12 6-12 Number of children using ST 3/6 3/4 2/4 3/5 Percentage of ST 50% 75% 50% 60% A noticeable difference exists between race groups with the usage of this process, with Black children demonstrating consistently higher usage of this process across all age ranges. Of further interest is the fact that for this sample of children this was the only process where Black children showed consistently higher percentages of usage scores across all ages, than White children. Finally, Table 16 depicts the Velar Fronting process pattern exhibited by the sample of children studied. Table 16 Children Exhibiting Velar Fronting Process (VF) White Children Black Children Age range 5-0 to 5-3 5-4 to 5-8 5-9 to 5-12 6-2 to 6-12 Number of children using VF 0/7 2/10 1/8 Percentage of VF 0% 20% 13% 2/12 17% Age range 5-0 to 5-3 5-4 to 5-8 5-9 to 5-12 6-9 to 6-12 Number of children using VF 0/ 1/4 0/4 1/5 Percentage of VF 0% 25% 0% 20% These data demonstrated that no characteristic differences were apparent between race groups across the age range in this study. The only age range that showed noticeable differences in percentage scores were the 5-9 to 5-12 range, where White children showed some use of this process, while Black children showed none. Assimilation Process Assimilation processes are sound changes in which one sound becomes more similar to another. It was evidenced in this sample that both Black and White children were using progressive assimilation, which is classified as a sound which causes the assimilation to preceded the affected sound. Table 16 contains the error patterns of Assimilation evidenced by the children sampled. Table 17 Children exhibiting Assimilation Process (AS) White Children Age 5-0 5-4 5-9 6-2 range to to to to 5-3 5-8 5-12 6-12 Number of children using AS 1/7 0/10 0/8 0/12 Black Percentage of AS 14% 0% 0% 0% Age 5-0 5-4 5-9 6-9 Children range to to to to 5-3 5-8 5-12 6-12 Number of children using AS 2/6 0/4 0/4 1/5 Black children evidenced higher percentage of usage at the earliest (5-0 to 5-3) and latest (6-9 to 6-12) age ranges. Percentage of usage among Black children at the earliest age range is nearly twice that Percentage of AS 33% 0% 0% 20% of White children of the same age. In addition, it is interesting to note that Black children are still evidencing this process at the latest age range, while the process is totally absent from White children in the same age range. Although the developmental data on phonological processing indicates that "normal" children no longer evidenced unstressed syllable deletion, final consonant deletion, velar fronting, and consonant assimilation after age three (Stoel Gammon & Dunn, 1985), the children sampled in this study were still evidencing these and other processes well into their fifth and sixth year. The pattern of phonological process usage often differed by race. Black children did not evidence final consonant deletion, but in contrast White children did exhibit this process from ages 5-4 to 5-12. Black children exhibited the process of assimilation at ages 5-0 to 5-4, and again occurring at age 6-2. White children evidenced this process only at ages 5-0 to 5-3. Black and White children were relatively equal in pattern distribution distribution velar fronting and unstressed syllable deletion. Conclusion The results of this comparative analysis of developmental realization of canonical form, phonetic features of manner and place, and phonological processes, in five and six-year-old White and Black children revealed that these children produced essentially the same type of errors, but that Black children produced some error types, especially at ages 6-9 to 6-12, that characterized Black English phonology. The apparent influence of Black English on developmental consonants production accounted for only a slightly different error pattern among place and manner, and phonological processing features. Despite dialectal background, the five and six-year old Black and White children in this sample produced the same developmental errors. These findings agreed with those of Seymour & Seymour (1981) who conducted a similar study and found that consonant features that contrast between Black English and Standard English were undifferentiated from developmental errors typical of white children who range in age from three to eight years of age (Ingram, 1976; and Templin, 1957). Consequently, articulation testing of Black children who have not yet mastered the adult Black English system would likely yield developmental errors consistent with normative data on standard American English. Indeed, Owens (1988) suggests that BE phonological features are not likely to emerge until around the age that Black children enter school when peer interaction becomes an important source of dialectal input. Nonetheless, it is hypothesized that some differences in the speech of young Black children and White children might be significantly greater than this study revealed, especially if spontaneous speech samples from naturalistic settings of the two groups were analyzed in a fashion similar to the one presented in this study. Single word utterances are not believed to capture the extent to which canonical form and phonological processing might differ in these two groups. The single word elicitation technique of many articulation test formats ignores certain important dynamics of articulation such as co­ articulation, prosody, situational, and linguistic variance. The use of the formal articulation format in gathering the data, however, was meaningful in that the researcher was able to assess and contrast consonantal responses in a typical testing procedure. The similarity of error types among children, and the comparability of articulatory performance with existing normative data, suggest that Black and White childrens' consonantal productions in single word responses in the 5- to 6-year-old range may not be significantly different. The speech-language pathologist assessing single word articulatory proficiency in African-American children should be mindful that preschool Black children are likely to produce developmental errors consistent with those of standard American English. Not until the latter part of the sixth year should features of BE phonology be expected to emerge. References Brenneise-Sarshad, R., Nicholas, L, & Brookshire, R (1991). Effects of apparent listener knowledge and picture stimuli on aphasic and non­ brain damaged speakers narrative discourse. JSHR 34(1), 168-176. Boyd, F., & Vaughn-Cooke, A F. (1983). Improving language assessment in minority children. ASHA, 22, 29-33. Brecht, M., Woodward, ]., Bonett, D. (1986). Liner Models for Experimental Designs. Los Angeles, CA Department of Psychology, UCLA. Cole, L (1983) Implications of the position on soical dialects. ASHA, 25, (9), 25-27. Cole, L, & Taylor, 0., (1990). Performance of working class AfricanAmerican children on three tests of articulation. ASHA, 21,171-176. Committee on Language, Speech and Hearing Services in the Schools (1982). Definitions: Communicative disorders and variations. ASHA 24 (ID, 949-950. Committee on the Status of Racial Minorities (1983). Social dialects position paper. ASHA 22 (U, 45. Committee on Status of Racial Minorities (1987). Social dialects position Paper. ASHA 22 (1), 45. Dillard J. L (1972). Blank Fnglish. New York: Random House. Edwards, M., & Shriberg, L (1983) in Communicative Disorders San Diego: College Hill Press. Goldman, R. & Fristoe, M. (1986). Goldman Fristoe Test of Articulation - : American Guidance Service. Grunwell, P. (1982) Clinical Phonology. Aspen, Rockville, MD Haelsig, Pv & Madison, C (1986) A study of phonological processes exhibited by 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 17, 107-114. Haynes, W., & Moran, M. (1989). A cross sectional development study of final consonant production in southern Black children from Services in Schools, 20, 400-406. Hodson, B. & Paden, E. (1981) Phonological processing patterns which characterized unintelligible and intelligible speech in early childhood. Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46, 369-373. Ingram, D. (1976) Phonological disability in children. New York: Labov, W. (1972). language in the inner city: Studies in the Black English Vernacular. Philadelphia: University of Penn. Press. Maxwell, C., & Bray, S. (1986). Robustness of the quasi-F through violations of spherecity Psychological BulletioiL 22, 416-421. Moulton, W. (1976). The sounds of Black English. In D. Harrison & T. Trabasso (Eds.), Black English a seminar (pp. 149-170). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Owens, R (1988). Language Development An Introduction, Geneseo, New York Merrill Publishing Company. Pendergast, K, Dickey, Stalley E., Selman, J. W., & Sorder, A. L (1969). Photo Articulation Test. Illinois: Interstate. Seymour, H. N., & Miller-Jones, D. (1981). Language and cognitive assessment of Black children. Speech and language: Advances in basic research and practice, 6, 203-263. Seymour, H., & Seymour, C, (1981). Black English and Standard American English contrasts in consonantal development of four and five-year old children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 2, 274-280. Shriberg L (1986) Programs to Examine Phonological Evaluation of Stockman, I. (1986). Language acquisition in culturally diverse populations: The Black child as a case study. In O. Taylor (Ed.). Nature of Communication disorders in culturally & linguistically diverse populations (pp. 117-156). San Diego, CA College-Hill Press. Taylor, O. (1986). The challenge of assessing the language of nonmainstreamed speakers. In O. Taylor (Ed.). Treatment of. communication Disorders in culturally and linguistically diverse populations (pp. 23-48). San Diego, CA College-Hill Press. Templin, M. C., & Darley, F. L. (1969). Articulation. (2nd ed.). Iowa City: Bureau of Educational Research and Service, Division of Extension and University Services. University of Iowa. Terrell, S. L, & Terrell, F. (1983) Distinguishing linguistic differences from disorders: The past, present, and future of nonbiased assessment. Topics in language Disorders, 3,1-7. Vaughn-Cooke, A F. (1980). Evaluating the language of Black English speakers: Implications of the Ann Arbor decision. In M. F. Whiteman (Ed.), Reactions to Ann Arbor decision: Vernacular Black English and education. Washington, DC Center for Applied Linguistics. Wolfram, W. & Fasold, K W. (1977) The study of social dialects In American English. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall ! APPENDIX A < Characteristics of Black English Phonology I. Plosives: At the end of a syllable, voiced stops, /b/, /d/, and /g/, are pronounced as the corresponding voiceless stops /p/, /1/, and /k/. This does not mean that bud and butt, and cab and cap sound alike in BEV. They are distinguished by length of the vowel, being held slightly longer when the following sound is voiced. For example, the "u" in bud is pronounced longer while the "u" in butt, could be pronounced /A/. IL Nasals: In Black English Vernacular there is a tendency for nasals not to be realized in the final position. For example, /com/ becomes /co/. Also, the "ng" becomes /n/ in the medial and final position so that sing and sin may sound alike, and in an unstressed syllable, as in swimmin for swimming or buyin for buying. III. Fricatives: In Black English Vernacular, the voiced "th" (/$/) becomes /d/ in the initial position, so that then and den sound alike. Medially, the voiced "th" (/$/) is realized as /d/ or /v/, so that other and udder or either and eva sound alike. Similarly, the voiceless "th" (/"0”/) in the initial position is realized as /f /or /v/ so that think becomes fink or vink. Medially the voiceless "th" ( ft /) is realized as /f/, making toothbrush into toofbrush. In the final position, the voiceless "th" (/ft/) changes to /f/ or/t/, making fourth to fort. IV. liquids: /r/ In Black English Vernacular, /r / is not realized between two vowels, so that Carol is realized as Ca'ol. Also, /r/ is not realized in the final position or preceding a consonant, thus bar is realized as bah. The liquid /!/ may not be realized before /t/,/d/,or/p/, therefore, help is realized as hep, and colt is realized as code. Also, /l/ may not be realized in the final positions, thus toll and toe may sound alike. V. Syllable Reduction: Word final consonant clusters in a stop can be reduced when both phonemes belong to a base word: tes (test), des (desk), han (hand), and buil (build). Words ending in a consonant are reduced when grammatical suffix-ed is added, so that rubbed becomes rub.